Article published In:
Diagrammatic Reasoning
Edited by Riccardo Fusaroli and Kristian Tylén
[Pragmatics & Cognition 22:2] 2014
► pp. 187205
References (40)
Baber, C., Parekh, M., Cengiz, T.G. (2014). Tool use as distributed cognition: How tools help, hinder and define manual skill. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, Article 1161, 1–16Google Scholar
Bilalić, M., McLeod, P., & Gobet, F. (2008). Why good thoughts block better ones: The mechanism of the pernicious Einstellung (set) effect. Cognition, 1081, 652–661. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bolland, B.R. (2011). Distributed cognition as a lens to understand the effects of scaffolds: The role of transfer of responsibility. Educational Psychology Review, 231, 577–600. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Evans, J. St. B.T. (in press). Reasoning, biases and dual processes: The lasting impact of Wason (1960). Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology.
Fleck, J.I., & Weisberg, R.W. (2013). Insight versus analysis: Evidence for diverse methods in problem solving. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 251, 436–463. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gale, M., & Ball, L.J. (2006). Dual-goal facilitation in Wason’s 2-4-6 task: What mediates successful rule discovery? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 591, 873–885. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gilhooly, K.J., & Fioratou, E. (2009). Executive functions in insight versus non-insight problem solving: An individual differences approach. Thinking and Reasoning, 151, 355–376. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Giere, R. (2006). Scientific perspectivism. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Järvilehto, T. (1998). The theory of the organism-environment system: I. Description of the theory. Integrative Physiological and Behavioral Science, 331, 321–334. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jensen, J. (1960). On the Einstellung effect in problem solving. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 11, 163–168. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kirsh, D. (1995). Complementary strategies: Why we use our hands when we think. In J.M. Moore & J.L. Lehman (Eds.), Proceedings of the Seventeenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 212–217). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.Google Scholar
. (2009). Projection, problem space and anchoring. In N. Taatgen, H. van Rijn, J. Nerbonne & L. Schomaker (Eds.), Proceedings of the Thirty-First Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. (pp. 2310–2315). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
. (2013). Thinking with external representation. In S.J. Cowley, & F. Vallée-Tourangeau (Eds.), Cognition beyond the brain: Computation, interactivity and human artifice (pp. 171–194). London: Springer-Verlag. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Knoblich, G., Ohlsson, S., & Raney, G.E. (2001). An eye movement study of insight problem solving. Memory & Cognition, 291, 1000–1009. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Knoblich, G., Ohlsson, S., Haider, H., & Rhenius, D. (1999). Constraint relaxation and chunk decomposition in insight problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 251, 1534–1555. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Luchins, A.S. (1942). Mechanization in problem solving: The effect of Einstellung. Psychological Monograph, 541 (Whole No. 248). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Maglio, P.P., Matlock, T., Raphaely, D., Chernicky, B., & Kirsh, D. (1999). Interactive skills in Scrabble. In M. Hahn, & S.C. Stoness (Eds.), Proceedings of the Twenty-First Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 326–330). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.Google Scholar
Malafouris, L. (2013). How things shape the mind: A theory of material engagement. London: The MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mayer, R.E. (1995). The search for insight: Grappling with Gestalt psychology’s unanswered questions. Insight and problem solving. In R.J. Sternberg and J.E. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of insight (pp. 3–32). Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Ohlsson, S. (1992). Information-processing explanations of insight and related phenomena. In M.T. Keane & K.J. Gilhooly (Eds.), Advances in the psychology of thinking (pp. 1–44). Hemel Hempstead, UK: Harvester Wheatsheaf.Google Scholar
Ohlsson, S. (2011). Deep learning: How the mind overrides experience. New York: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Popper, K. (1935/1992). The logic of scientific discovery. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Ross, L.D., Amabile, T.A., & Steinmetz, J.L. (1977). Social roles, social control, and biases in social-perception processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 351, 485–494. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Segal, E. (2004). Incubation in insight problem solving. Creativity Research Journal, 161, 141–148. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Steffensen, S.V. (2013). Human interactivity: Problem-solving, solution probing and verbal patterns in the wild. In S.J. Cowley, & F. Vallée-Tourangeau (Eds.), Cognition beyond the brain: Computation, interactivity and human artifice (pp. 195–221). London: Springer-Verlag. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2015). Distributed language and dialogism: Notes on non-locality, sense-making and interactivity. Language Sciences, 501, 105–119. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Simon, H.A. (1996). The sciences of the artificial (3rd edition). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Turvey, M.T., & Shaw, R.E. (1999). Ecological foundations of cognition I: Symmetry and specificity of animal-environment systems. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6 (11-12), 95–110.Google Scholar
Vallée-Tourangeau, F. (2014). Insight, interactivity and materiality. Pragmatics & Cognition, 221, 27–44.Google Scholar
Vallée-Tourangeau, F., Austin, N.G., & Rankin, S. (1995). Inducing a rule in Wason’s 2-4-6 task: A test of the information-quantity and goal-complementarity hypotheses. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48A1, 895–914. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vallée-Tourangeau, F., Euden, G., & Hearn, V. (2011). Einstellung defused: Interactivity and mental set. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 641, 1889–1895. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vallée-Tourangeau, F., & Payton, T. (2008). Graphical representation fosters discovery in the 2-4-6 task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 611, 625–640. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vallée-Tourangeau, F., Steffensen, S.V., Vallée-Tourangeau, G, & Makri, A. (2015). Insight and cognitive ecosystems. In D. C. Noelle, R. Dale, A.S. Warlaumont>, J. Yoshimi, T. Matlock, C.D. Jennings, & P.P. Maglio (Eds.), Proceedings of the Thirty-seventh Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 2457–2462).Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Vallée-Tourangeau, G., & Vallée-Tourangeau, F. (2014). The spatio-temporal dynamics of systemic thinking. Cybernetics and Human Knowing, 211, 113–127.Google Scholar
Van Der Henst, J.-B., Rossi, S., & Schroyens, W. (2002). When participants are not misled they are not so bad after all: A pragmatic analysis of a rule discovery task. In W.D. Gray & C.D. Schunn (Eds.), Proceedings of the Twenty-Fourth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 902–908). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.Google Scholar
Van Stockum, C., & DeCaro, M.S. (2013). The path less taken: When working memory capacity constrains insight. In M. Knauff, M. Pauen, N. Sebanz, & I. Wachsmuth (Eds.), Proceedings of the Thirty-Fifth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 3633–3638). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Wason, P.C. (1960). On the failure to eliminate hypotheses in a conceptual task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 121, 129–140. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Weller, A., Villejoubert, G., & Vallée-Tourangeau, F. (2011). Interactive insight problem solving. Thinking & Reasoning, 171, 429–439. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wertheimer, M. (1959). Productive thinking. New York: Harper & Brothers Publishers.Google Scholar
Zhang, J., & Norman, D.A. (1994). Representations in distributed cognitive tasks. Cognitive Science, 181, 87–122. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by (7)

Cited by seven other publications

Ross, Wendy
2023. Serendipitous Cognition—The Systematic Consideration of the Accidental Genesis of New Ideas. In Serendipity Science,  pp. 167 ff. DOI logo
Bianchi, Ivana, Elena Capitani, Erika Branchini, Ugo Savardi & Roberto Burro
2022. Naïve intuitions about what constitutes “an opposite process”. Journal of Cognitive Psychology 34:3  pp. 410 ff. DOI logo
Ross, Wendy & Frédéric Vallée-Tourangeau
2021. Rewilding Cognition: Complex Dynamics in Open Experimental Systems. Journal of Trial and Error 2:1  pp. 30 ff. DOI logo
Vallée-Tourangeau, Frédéric & Gaëlle Vallée-Tourangeau
2020. Mapping systemic resources in problem solving. New Ideas in Psychology 59  pp. 100812 ff. DOI logo
Vallée-Tourangeau, Frédéric & Gaëlle Vallée-Tourangeau
2020. Insight. In The Palgrave Encyclopedia of the Possible,  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo
Vallée-Tourangeau, Frédéric & Gaëlle Vallée-Tourangeau
2022. Insight. In The Palgrave Encyclopedia of the Possible,  pp. 779 ff. DOI logo
Fusaroli, Riccardo & Kristian Tylén
2014. Diagrammatic reasoning. Pragmatics & Cognition 22:2  pp. 183 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 21 september 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.