A review of the current literature shows that by the age of two and a half (and probably earlier), children have already acquired a rich working knowledge of human intentionality and goal-directed action (Stein & Albro 1997: 7; Mandler 1998). The paper focuses on the ways in which children use this knowledge to tell stories from pictures. The story is the description of the actions performed by animate actors. We distinguish the main actors (protagonists in the narrative line) and the background actors (participants in the narrative field) who can observe and interpret what is going on in the main action. So the narrative text contains not only the action presented by the story-teller (landscape of action) but also how this action is interpreted by the story characters (landscape of consciousness). They are all thinking minds who can think similarly or differently about the plot. And the narrator uses characters’ minds to produce different representations of the story (Bokus 1998, 2000). The narrator can confront one interpretation with another, and a) makes choices of the “true” representation of the main action (in doing this the child plays the role of the omniscient and omnipresent story-teller who is directly in touch with the ontology of the story), or b) presents a possible but not a certain story reality (the listener is not told how things are but rather how they seem to be). Therefore we can speak about the interplay of the narrator’s mind and the minds of story characters in a kind of internal narrator’s dialogue. The storyteller creates different minds and alternative ways of interpreting the main action. Also shown are examples of such inter-mind phenomena in the stories told by preschool children.
Abbeduto, L., and S. Rosenberg (1985) Children’s knowledge of the presuppositions of know and other verbs. Journal of Child Language 121: 621-641.
Astington, J.W. (1990) Narrative and the child’s theory of mind. In B.K. Britton, & A.D. Pellegrini (eds.), Narrative thought and narrative language. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 151-171.
Astington, J.W. (1993) The child’s discovery of the mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Bakhtin, M. (1984) Problems of Dostoevski’s poetics. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
Bamberg, M. (ed.) (1997) Narrative development: Six approaches. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. BoP
Bamberg, M., and R. Damrad-Frye (1991) On the ability to provide evaluative comments: Further explorations of children’s narrative competence. Journal of Child Language 181: 689-710.
Bartsch, K., and H.M. Wellman (1995) Children talk about the mind. New York: Oxford University Press.
Bokus, B. (1991) Children’s pragmatic knowledge of narrative tasks. In J. Verschueren (ed.), Pragmatics at issue. Vol. 1 (Selected papers of the International Pragmatics Conference, Antwerp – 1987). Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, pp. 13-28.
Bokus, B. (1996a) Action and consciousness: Duality of children’s narrative construction. Paper presented at the VIIth International Congress for the Study of Child Language (Istanbul, July 14–19, 1996).
Bokus, B. (1996b) Narrative space structuring at the preschool age (Findings on monologic and dialogic discourse. In C.E. Johnson & J.H.V. Gilbert (eds.), Children’s Language, vol. 9. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 197-207.
Bokus, B. (1998) Action and its representation in the minds of story characters. Findings from children’s discourse. Psychology of Language and Communication 2.2: 63-77.
Bokus, B. (2000) Światy fabuły w narracji dziecięcej (Story worlds in child narration). Warsaw: Energeia.
Britton, B.K., and A.D. Pellegrini (eds.) (1990) Narrative thought and narrative language. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. BoP
Bruner, J. (1986) Actual minds, possible worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Bruner, J. (1990) Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Donaldson, M. (1978). Children’s minds. New York: Norton.
Eco, U. (1994) Lector in fabula. Warszawa: PIW.
Ely, R., A. McCabe, A. Wolf, and G. Melzi (2000) The story behind the story: Gathering narrative data from children. In L. Menn, & N.B. Ratner (eds.), Methods for studying language production. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 249-269.
Flavell, J.H., and P.H. Miller (1998) Social cognition. In D. Kuhn, & R. Siegler (eds.), Cognition, perception, and language. Vol. II of W. Damon (Gen.ed.), Handbook of child psychology. New York: Wiley, pp. 851-898.
Fox, R. (1991) Developing awareness of mind reflected in children’s narrative writing. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 91: 281-298.
Gopnik, A., and A.N. Meltzoff (1997) Words, thoughts, and theories. Cambridge MA: MIT Press. BoP
Halliday, M.A.K., and R. Hasan (1976) Cohesion in English. London: Longman. BoP
Hermans, H.J.M. (1997) Self-narrative in the life course: A contextual approach. In M. Bamberg (ed.), Narrative development: Six approaches. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 223-264.
Hudson, J.A., and L.R. Shapiro (1991) From kowing to telling: The development of children’s scripts, stories, and personal narratives. In A. McCabe, & C. Peterson (eds.), Developing narrative structure. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 89-136.
Kielar-Turska, M. (1999) The inner landscape of characters in stories told by children. Psychology of Language and Communication 3.2: 49-56.
Labov, W., and J. Waletzky (1967) Narrative analysis: Oral versions of personal experience. In J. Helm (ed.), Essays on the verbal and visual arts. Seattle: University of Washington Press, pp. 12-44.
Lewis, C., N.H. Freeman, C. Hagestadt, and H. Douglas (1994) Narrative access and production in preschoolers’ false belief reasoning. Cognitive Development 91: 397-424.
Lucariello, J. (1990) Canonicality and consciousness in child narrative. In B.K. Britton, & A.D. Pellegrini (eds.), Narrative thought and narrative language. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 149.
Lyons, J. (1977) Semantics, vol. 2. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Macnamara, J., E. Baker, and C. Olson (1976) Four-year-olds’ understanding of pretend, forget and know: Evidence for propositional operations. Child Development 471: 62-70.
Mandler, J.M. (1998) Representation. In D. Kuhn, & R.S. Siegler (eds.), Cognition, perception, and language: Vol. 2, W. Damon (ed.),
Handbook of child psychology
. New York: Wiley, pp. 255-281.
Musatti, T. (1986) Early peer relations: The perspectives of Piaget and Vygotsky. In E.C. Mueller, & C.R. Cooper (eds.), Process and outcome in peer relationships. Orlando: Academic Press, pp. 25-53.
Nicolopoulou, A., and E.S. Richner (1999a) From actors to characters to persons: The development of character representation in young children’s narratives. Poster presented at the 1999 Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development. Albuquerque (April 15-18, 1999), (work in progress), NM.
Nicolopoulou, A., and E.S. Richner (1999b) The development of main character in young children’s narratives. Paper presented at the VIIIth Congress for the Study of Child Language, San Sebastian (July 12–16, 1999), NM.
Pillow, B.H. (1989) Early understanding of perception as a source of knowledge. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 471: 116-129.
Pratt, C., and P. Bryant (1990) Young children understand that looking leads to knowing. Child Development 611: 973-982.
Searle, J.R. (1983) Intentionality: An essay in the philosophy of mind. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Searle, J.R. (2002) Consciousness and language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Shields, M. (1978) The child as psychologist: Construing the social world. In A. Lock (ed.), Action, gesture, and symbol: The emergence of language. London: Academic Press, pp. 529-561.
Shugar, G.W. (1976) Text-constructing with an adult. A form of child activity during early language acquisition. In D. Drachman (ed.), Akten des 1. Salzburger Kolloquiums über Kindersprache. Tübingen: Verlag Gunter Narr, pp. 343-356.
Shugar, G.W. (1998) Entry into narration: Uses of operation of reference. Psychology of Language and Communication 2.2: 7-16.
Stein, N.L., and E.R. Albro (1997) Building complexity and coherence: Children’s use of goal-structured knowledge in telling stories. In M. Bamberg (ed.), Narrative development: Six approaches. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 5-44.
Taylor, M. (1996) A theory of mind perspective on social development. In R. Gelman, & T.K.-F. Au (eds.), Perceptual and cognitive development. San Diego, CA: Academic Press, pp. 283-329.
Tomasello, M. (1999) The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Tomaszewski, T. (1975) Człowiek i otoczenie (Man and environment). In T. Tomaszewski (ed.), Psychologia. Warsaw: PWN, pp. 13-36.
Wertsch, J.V. (1991) Voices of the mind. A sociocultural approach to mediated action. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.
2020. Developmental Psycholinguistics: Old Questions, New Answers. Psychology of Language and Communication 24:1 ► pp. 70 ff.
Otwinowska, Agnieszka, Karolina Mieszkowska, Marta Białecka-Pikul, Marcin Opacki & Ewa Haman
2020. Retelling a model story improves the narratives of Polish-English bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism 23:9 ► pp. 1083 ff.
Milanowicz, Anna
2019. A Short Etude on Irony in Storytelling. Psychology of Language and Communication 23:1 ► pp. 14 ff.
Jakubowska, Joanna, Marta Szpak & Marta Białecka-Pikul
2018. How Do Parents Use Mental State Language During Narration To Their 2- and 4-Year-Old Children?. Psychology of Language and Communication 22:1 ► pp. 516 ff.
Ovchinnikova, Irina
2018. Effect of the content complexity on hesitations in adolescents’ narratives. Psychology of Language and Communication 22:1 ► pp. 1 ff.
2018. Right Cerebral Hemisphere Language and Communication Functions in Females and Males with Autism Spectrum Disorders and Normal Intelligence. Psychology of Language and Communication 22:1 ► pp. 372 ff.
Zevenbergen, Andrea Angell, Ewa Haman & Jason Andrew Zevenbergen
2018. “Do You Remember Going to the Beach?”: References to Internal States in Polish and American Mother-Preschooler Shared Narratives. Psychology of Language and Communication 22:1 ► pp. 441 ff.
2016. Evaluative language used by Mandarin-Chinese-speaking dyads in personal narratives: age and socioeconomic differences. Early Child Development and Care 186:7 ► pp. 1017 ff.
Banasik, Natalia
2013. Non-literal speech comprehension in preschool children – an example from a study on verbal irony. Psychology of Language and Communication 17:3 ► pp. 309 ff.
Veneziano, Edy
2010. Utilisations du langage et développement de la capacité à maîtriser plusieurs points de vue chez l’enfant. In La pluralité interprétative,
Veneziano, Edy
2016. The Development of Narrative Discourse in French by 5 to 10 Years Old Children: Some Insights from a Conversational Interaction Method. In Written and Spoken Language Development across the Lifespan [Literacy Studies, 11], ► pp. 141 ff.
Veneziano, Edy
2017. Talking About the Non-Literal: Internal States and Explanations in Child-Constructed Narratives. Psychology of Language and Communication 21:1 ► pp. 133 ff.
Veneziano, Edy
2019. Conversationally and Monologically-Produced Narratives: A Complex Story of Horizontal Décalages. Psychology of Language and Communication 23:1 ► pp. 85 ff.
Berman, Ruth A.
2009. Trends in Research on Narrative Development. In Language Acquisition, ► pp. 294 ff.
Veneziano, Edy & Christian Hudelot
2009. Explaining Events in Narratives: The Impact of Scaffolding in 4 To 12 Year Old Children. Psychology of Language and Communication 13:1
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 28 october 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.