Article published In:
Issues in Humour Cognition
Edited by Marta Dynel
[Review of Cognitive Linguistics 16:1] 2018
► pp. 118
References
Antonopoulou, E.
(2002) A cognitive approach to literary humour devices: Translating Raymond Chandler. In J. Vandaele (Ed.), Humour and translation. Special issue of The Translator , 8(2), 235–257.Google Scholar
Antonopoulou, E. A., & Nikiforidou, K.
(2009) Deconstructing verbal humour with construction grammar. In G. Brone & J. Vandaele (Eds.), Cognitive poetics: Goals, gains and gaps (pp. 289–314). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Antonopoulou, E., Nikiforidou, K., & Tsakona, V.
(2015) Construction grammar and discoursal incongruity. In G. Brône, K. Feyaerts, & T. Veale (Eds.), Cognitive linguistics and humor research (pp. 13–48). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Apter, M. J.
(1982) The experience of motivation: The theory of psychological reversals. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Attardo, S.
(1994) Linguistic theories of humor. New York: Mouton.Google Scholar
(2015) Humorous metaphors. In G. Brône, K. Feyaerts, & T. Veale (Eds.), Humor and cognitive linguistics (pp. 91–110). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Attardo, S., & Raskin, V.
(2017) Linguistics and humour theory. In S. Attardo (Ed.), The Routledge handbook of language and humor (pp. 49–63). Oxon: Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Aymone, A.
(2007) Interview with Victor Raskin. In D. Popa, & S. Attardo (Eds.), New approaches to the linguistics of humour (pp. 217–225). Galati: Editura Academica.Google Scholar
Barcelona, A.
(2003) The case for a metonymic basis of pragmatic inferencing: Evidence from jokes and funny anecdotes. In K. Panther & L. Thornburg (Eds.), Metonymy and pragmatic inferencing (pp. 81–102). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bergen, B. K.
(2003) To awaken a sleeping giant: Cognition and culture in September 11 political cartoons. In M. Achard & S. Kemmer (Eds.), Language, culture, and mind (pp. 23–35). Stanford, CA: CSLI.Google Scholar
Bergen, B. K., & Binsted, K.
(2015) Embodied grammar and humor. In G. Brône, K. Feyaerts, & T. Veale (Eds.), Cognitive linguistics meets humor research: Current trends and new developments (pp. 49–68). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brandt, L.
(2003) Humor and meaning construction in everyday speech: A mental space analysis. Paper presented at the 8th International Cognitive Linguistics Conference , University of La Rioja, Spain.
Brône, G.
(2008) Hyper- and misunderstanding in interactional humor. Journal of Pragmatics, 40(12), 2027–2061. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012) Humour and irony in cognitive pragmatics. In H. Schmid (Ed.), Cognitive pragmatics (pp. 463–504). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017) Cognitive linguistics and humor research. In S. Attardo (Ed.), The Routledge handbook of language and humor (pp. 250–266). Oxon: Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brône, G., & Coulson, S.
(2010) On the cognitive processing of deliberate ambiguity in newspaper headlines: The case of double grounding. Discourse Processes, 471, 212–236. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brône, G., & Feyaerts, K.
(2004) Assessing the SSTH and GTVH: A view from cognitive linguistics. Humor: International Journal of Humor Research, 17(4), 361–372. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brône, G., Feyaerts, K., & Veale, T.
(2006) Introduction: Cognitive linguistic approaches to humor. Humor: International Journal of Humor Research, 19(3), 203–228. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bryant, G., & Gibbs, R.
(2015) Behavioral complexities of ironic humor. In G. Brône, K. Feyaerts, & T. Veale (Eds.), Cognitive linguistics and humor research (pp. 147–166). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Canestrari, C., & Bianchi, I.
(2012) Perception of contrariety in jokes. Discourse Processes, 491, 539–564. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2013) From perception of contraries to humorous incongruities. In M. Dynel (Ed.), Developments in linguistic humour theory (pp. 3–24). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Canestrari, C., Dionigi, A., & Zuczkowski, A.
(2014) Humor understanding and knowledge. Language and Dialogue, 4(2), 261–283. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cori, V., Canestrari, C., & Bianchi, I.
(2016) The perception of contrariety and the processing of verbal irony. Gestalt Theory, 38(2–3), 253–266.Google Scholar
Coulson, S.
(1996) The Menendez brothers virus: Analogical mapping in blended spaces. In A. E. Goldberg (Ed.), Conceptual structure, discourse, and language (pp. 67–81). Palo Alto: CSLI.Google Scholar
(2000) Semantic leaps: Frame-shifting and conceptual blending in meaning construction. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
(2005a) “What’s so funny?”: Cognitive semantics and jokes. Cognitive Psychopathology, 21, 67–78.Google Scholar
(2005b) Extemporaneous blending: conceptual integration in humorous discourse from talk radio. Style, 391, 107–122.Google Scholar
(2005c) Sarcasm and the space structuring model. In S. Coulson, & B. Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk (Eds.), The literal and the nonliteral in language and thought (pp. 129–144). Berlin: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
(2015) Frame-shifting and frame semantics: Joke comprehension on the space structuring model. In: G. Brône, K. Feyaerts, & T. Veale (Eds.), Cognitive linguistics and humor research (pp. 167–190). Berlin, Germany: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Coulson, S., & Kutas, M.
(1998) Frame-shifting and sentential integration. Cognitive Science Technical Report, 98.02. UCSD.Google Scholar
Coulson, S. & Kutas, M.
(2001) Getting it: Human event-related brain response to jokes in good and poor comprehenders . Neuroscience Letters, 3161, 71–74. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Coulson, S., & Severens, E.
(2007) Hemispheric asymmetry and pun comprehension: When cowboys have sore calves. Brain and Language, 1001, 172–187. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Coulson, S., Urbach, T. P., & Kutas, M.
(2006) Looking back: Joke comprehension and the space structuring model. Humor: International Journal of Humor Research, 19(3), 229–250. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
de Jongste, H.
(2013) Negotiating humorous intent. In M. Dynel (Ed.), Developments in linguistic humour theory (pp. 179–210). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2016) Mental models and humorous intent. Journal of Pragmatics, 951, 107–119. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017) Culture and incongruity in The Office (UK). Language & Communication, 551, 88–99. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dynel, M.
(2009a) Humorous garden-paths: A pragmatic-cognitive study. Newcastle: Cambridge Scholars Publishing.Google Scholar
(2009b) Metaphor is a birthday cake: Metaphor as the source of humour. metaphorik.de, 171, 27–48.Google Scholar
(2011) Blending the incongruity-resolution model and the conceptual integration theory: The case of blends in pictorial advertising. International Review of Pragmatics, 3(1), 59–83. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2013) When does irony tickle the hearer?: Towards capturing the characteristics of humorous irony. In M. Dynel (Ed.), Developments in linguistic humour theory (pp. 105–144). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2014) Isn’t it ironic?: Defining the scope of humorous irony. Humor: International Journal of Humor Research, 27(4), 619–639. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017) Academics vs. American scriptwriters vs. Academics: A battle over the etic and emic “sarcasm” and “irony” labels. Language & Communication, 551, 69–87. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2018) Irony, deception and humour: Seeking the truth about overt and covert untruthfulness. Berlin: de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fauconnier, G.
(1985) Mental spaces: Aspects of meaning construction in natural language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
(1997) Mappings in thought and language. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fauconnier, G., & Turner, M.
(1998) Conceptual integration networks. Cognitive Science, 22(2), 133–187. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2002) The way we think: Conceptual blending and the mind’s hidden complexities. New York, NY: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Forabosco, G.
(1992) Cognitive aspects of the humour process: The concept of incongruity. Humor, 51, 9–26. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2008) Is the concept of incongruity still a useful construct for the advancement of humor research? Lodz Papers in Pragmatics, 41, 45–62. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gentner, D.
(1983) Structure-mapping: A theoretical framework for analogy. Cognitive Science, 7(2), 155–170. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, R.
(2005) Embodiment and cognitive science. New York: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017) Metaphor wars: Conceptual metaphor in human life. New York: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, R., & Colston, H.
(Eds.) (2007) Irony in language and thought: A cognitive science reader. New York: Erlbaum. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Giora, R.
(1991) On the cognitive aspects of the joke. Journal of Pragmatics, 16(5), 465–486. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1997) Understanding figurative and literal language: The graded salience hypothesis. Cognitive Linguistics, 71, 183–206. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2003) On our mind: Salience, context and figurative language. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011) Will anticipating irony facilitate it immediately? In M. Dynel (Ed.). The pragmatics of humour across discourse domains (pp. 19–31). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Giora, R. & Fein, O.
(1999) Irony comprehension: The graded salience hypothesis. Humor, 12(4), 425–436. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Giora, R., Fein, O., Kotler, N., & Shuval, N.
(2015) Know hope: Metaphor, optimal innovation and pleasure. In G. Brône, K. Feyaerts, & T. Veale (Eds.), Cognitive linguistics and humor research (pp. 129–146). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Giora, R., Fein, O., Kronrod, A., Elnatan, I., Shuval, N., & Zur, A.
(2004) Weapons of mass distraction: Optimal innovation and pleasure ratings. Metaphor and Symbol, 191, 115–141. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Giora, R., Givoni, S., Heruti, V., & Fein, O.
(2017) The role of defaultness in affecting pleasure: The optimal innovation hypothesis revisited. Metaphor & Symbol, 32(1), 1–18. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, A.
(1995) Constructions: A construction grammar approach to argument structure. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Hampe, B.
(Ed.) (2017) Metaphor: Embodied cognition and discourse. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hofstadter, D., & Gabora, L.
(1989) Frame blends. Humor, 21, 417–440.Google Scholar
Jabłońska-Hood, J.
(2015) A conceptual blending theory of humour: Selected British comedy productions in focus. Frankfurt: Peter Lang. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N.
(1983) Mental models: Towards a cognitive science of language, inference, and consciousness. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Kihara, Y.
(2005) The mental space structure of verbal irony. Cognitive Linguistics, 161, 513–530. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Koestler, A.
(1964) The act of creation. London: Hutchison.Google Scholar
Kotthoff, H.
(2006) Pragmatics of performance and the analysis of conversational humor. Humor: International Journal of Humor Research, 19(3), 271–304. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Langacker, R.
(1987) Foundations of cognitive grammar: Theoretical prerequisites. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
(2008) Cognitive grammar: A basic introduction. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Maier, N.
(1932) A gestalt theory of humour. British Journal of Psychology, 231, 69–74.Google Scholar
Martin, R.
(2007) The psychology of humor: An integrative approach. Burlington, MA: Elsevier. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mayerhofer, B.
(2013) Perspective clashing as a humour mechanism. In M. Dynel (Ed.), Developments in linguistic humour theory (pp. 211–234). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mayerhofer, B., Maier, K., & Schacht, A.
(2015) Priming interpretations: Contextual impact on the processing of garden path jokes. Discourse Processes, 53(8), 675–694. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mayerhofer, B., & Schacht, A.
(2013) Salience, accessibility, and humorous potential in the comprehension of garden path jokes: A probabilistic approach. In M. Dynel (Ed.), Developments in linguistic humour theory (pp. 341–366). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015) From incoherence to mirth: Neuro-cognitive processing of garden-path jokes. Frontiers in Psychology, 61, 550. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morreall, J.
(2010) Humor as cognitive play. Journal of Literary Theory, 3(2), 241–260.Google Scholar
Nerhardt, G.
(1976) Incongruity and funniness: Towards a new descriptive model. In A. J. Chapman, & H. C. Foot (Eds.), Humour and laughter: Theory, research and applications (pp. 55–62). London: Transaction Publishers.Google Scholar
Oring, E.
(1992) Jokes and their relations. Lexington: The University Press of Kentucky.Google Scholar
(2003) Engaging humor. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
Pálinkás, I.
(2014) Blending and folk theory in an explanation of irony. Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 12(1), 64–98. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Partington, A.
(2006) The linguistics of laughter: A corpus-assisted study of laughter-talk. London: Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pollio, H.
(1996) Boundaries in humor and metaphor. In S. Mio, & A. Katz (Eds.), Metaphor: Implications and applications (pp. 231–253). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Raskin, V.
(1985) Semantic mechanisms of humor. Dordrecht: D. Reidel.Google Scholar
Ritchie, D.
(2005) Frame-shifting in humor and irony. Metaphor and Symbol, 20(4), 275–294. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ritchie, G.
(2004) The linguistic analysis of jokes. London: Routledge. DOI logo DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2006) Reinterpretation and viewpoints. Humor, 19(3), 251–270. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rojo López, A. M.
(2002) Frame semantics and the translation of humour. Babel, 48(1), 34–77. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009) A cognitive approach to the translation of metonymy-based humor. Across Languages and Cultures, 10(1), 63–83. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Samermit, P., & Gibbs, R.
(2016) Humor, the body, and cognitive linguistics. Cognitive Linguistic Studies, 21, 32–49. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shelley, C.
(2001) The bicoherence theory of situational irony. Cognitive Science, 251, 775–818. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shultz, T.
(1972) The role of incongruity and resolution in children’s appreciation of cartoon humor. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 131, 456–477. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Suls, J.
(1972) A two-stage model for the appreciation of jokes and cartoons: An information processing analysis. In J. Goldstein & P. McGhee (Eds.), The psychology of humor (pp. 81–100). New York: Academic Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1983) Cognitive processes in humor appreciation. In P. McGhee & J. Goldstein (Eds.), Handbook of humor research, Vol. 1 (pp. 39–57). New York: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tourangeau, R., & Sternberg, R. J.
(1981) Aptness in metaphor. Cognitive Psychology, 13(1), 27–55. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Turner, M.
(2014) The origin of ideas: Blending, creativity, and the human spark. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
(2015) Blending in language and communication. In E. Dąbrowska & D. Divjak (Eds.), Handbook of cognitive linguistics (pp. 211–232). Berlin: Mouton De Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Uekermann, J., Daum, I., & Channon, S.
(2007) Toward a cognitive and social neuroscience of humor processing. Social Cognition, 25(4), 553–572. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van Dijk, T. A.
(2008) Discourse and context: A sociocognitive approach. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009) Society and discourse: How social contexts influence text and talk. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2014) Discourse and knowledge: A sociocognitive approach. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Veale, T.
(2008) Figure-ground duality in humour: A multi-modal perspective. Lodz Papers in Pragmatics, 4(1), 63–81. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Veale, T., Feyaerts, K., & Brône, G.
(2006) The cognitive mechanisms of adversarial humor. Humor: International Journal of Humor Research, 19(3), 305–340. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Veale, T., & Valitutti, A.
(2017) Tweet dreams are made of this: Appropriate incongruity in the dreamwork of language. Lingua, 1971, 141–153. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yus, F.
(2017) Incongruity-resolution cases in jokes. Lingua, 1971, 103–122. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 6 other publications

Attardo, Salvatore
2020. Humor. In Handbook of Pragmatics [Handbook of Pragmatics, ],  pp. 155 ff. DOI logo
Buján Navarro, Marta
2019. Humour in interaction and cognitive linguistics: critical review and convergence of approaches. Complutense Journal of English Studies 27  pp. 139 ff. DOI logo
Dynel, Marta
2020. Book review. Journal of Pragmatics 155  pp. 349 ff. DOI logo
Linares Bernabéu, Esther
2023. Co-constructing humour and gender identity in live stand-up comedy. In The Pragmatics of Humour in Interactive Contexts [Pragmatics & Beyond New Series, 335],  pp. 200 ff. DOI logo
Linares-Bernabéu, Esther
2021. Cognitive strategies and figurative language in subversive stand-up comedy: the case of trumping (Estrategias cognitivas y lenguaje figurativo en el monólogo humorístico subversivo: el caso de la baza lúdica). Studies in Psychology 42:2  pp. 428 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 13 april 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.