Article published In:
Review of Cognitive Linguistics
Vol. 18:2 (2020) ► pp.428457
References (94)
References
Alexander, R. D. (1987). The biology of moral systems. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Auracher, J. (2017). Sound iconicity of abstract concepts: Place of articulation is implicitly associated with abstract concepts of size and social dominance. PlosONE, 12(11), e0187196. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bergen, B. K. (2004). The psychological reality of phonesthemes. Language, 801, 290–311. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Berlin, B. (1994). Evidence for pervasive synesthetic sound symbolism in ethnozoological nomenclature. In L. Hinton, J. Nicols & J. Ohala (Eds.), Sound symbolism (pp. 76–103). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
(2006). The first congress of ethonozoological nomenclature. Journal of Royal Anthropological Institution, 121, 23–44. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boehm, C. (2012). Moral origins: The evolution of virtue, altruism, and shame. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Brown, R., & Ford, M. (1961). Address in American English. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 621, 375–385. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cassidy, K. W., Kelly, M. H., & Sharoni, L. J. (1999). Inferring gender from name phonology. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 1281, 362–381. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cutler, A., McQueen, J., & Robinson, K. (1990). Elizabeth and John: Sound patterns of men’s and women’s names. Journal of Linguistics, 261, 471–482. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crisinel, A.-S., & Spence, C. (2009). Implicit association between basic tastes and pitch. Neuroscience Letters 4641, 39–42. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dingemanse, M., Blasi, D. E., Lupyan, G., Christiansen, M. H., & Monaghan, P. (2015). Arbitrariness, iconicity and systematicity in language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(10), 603–615. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
D’Onofrio, A. (2014). Phonetic detail and dimensionality in sound-shape correspondences: Refining the bouba-kiki paradigm. Language and Speech, 571, 367–393. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fuchs, S., Savin, E., Solt, S., Ebert, C., & Krifca, M. (2019). Antonym adjective pairs and prosodic iconicity: Evidence from letter replications in an English blogger corpus. Linguistic Vanguard, 1–15. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Godoy, M. C., Filho, N. S. S., Souza, J. G. M., Alves, H., & Kawahara, S. (2019). Gotta name them all: an experimental study on the sound symbolism of Pokémon names in Brazilian Portuguese. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Haiman, J. (1980). The iconicity of grammar: Isomorphism and motivation. Language, 56(3), 515–540. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(Ed.). (1985). Iconicity in syntax. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hamano, S. (1986). The sound-symbolic system of Japanese (Doctoral dissertation). University of Florida.Google Scholar
Hayes, B., & Steriade, D. (2004). Introduction: The phonetic bases of phonological markedness. In B. Hayes, R. Kirchner, & D. Steriade (Eds.), Phonetically based phonology (pp. 1–33). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hinton, L., Nichols, J., & Ohala, J. (2006). Sound symbolism (2nd ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hockett, C. (1959). Animal “languages” and human language. Human Biology, 311, 32–39.Google Scholar
Iwasaki, N., Vinson, D. P., & Vigiliocco, G. (2007). What do English speakers know about gera-gera and yota-yota? A cross-linguistic investigation of mimetic words for laughing and walking. Japanese Language Education Around the Globe, 171, 53–78.Google Scholar
Jaeger, J. J. (1978). Speech aerodynamics and phonological universals. In J. Jaeger, A. Woodbury, F. Ackerman, C. Chiavello, O. Gensler, J. Kingston & K. Whistler (Eds.), Proceedings of Berkeley linguistic society 4 (pp. 311–325). Berkeley: Berkeley Linguistics Society. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jakobson, R. (1968). Child language, aphasia and phonological universals. (A. Keiler, Trans.). The Hague: Mouton de Gruyter. (Original work published 1941) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1969). Why “mama” and “papa”? In R. Jakobson (Ed.), Selected writings, vol 1: Phonological studies. The Hague & Paris: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Jespersen, O. (1922). Symbolic value of the vowel i . In Phonologica. selected papers in English, French and German (Vol. 11) (pp. 283–303). Copenhagen: Levin & Munksgaard.Google Scholar
Johnson, M. (1987). The body in the mind: The bodily basis of meaning, imagination, and reason. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jurafsky, D. (2014). The language of food: A linguist reads the menu. New York: W. W. Norton & Company.Google Scholar
Kawahara, S. (2017). Introducing phonetics through sound symbolism. Tokyo: Hitsuji Syobo.Google Scholar
(2020). Sound symbolism and theoretical phonology. Language and Linguistic Compass. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kawahara, S. & Breiss, C. (2020). Exploring the nature of cumulativitity through sound symbolism: Experimental studies of Pokémonastics with English speakers. Manuscript, Keio Unviersity and UCLA.Google Scholar
Kawahara, S., Isobe, M., Kobayashi, Y., Monou, T., & Okabe, R. (2018a). Acquisition of sound symbolic values of vowels and voiced obstruents by Japanese children: Using a Pokémonastics paradigm. Journal of the Phonetic Society of Japan, 221, 122–130.Google Scholar
Kawahara, S., & Kumagai, G. (2019a). Expressing evolution in Pokémon names: Experimental explorations. Journal of Japanese Linguistics, 35(1), 2–38. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kawahara, S. and Kumagai, G. (2019b). Inferring Pokémon types using sound symbolism: The effects of voicing and labiality. Journal of Phonetic Society of Japan, 23(2), 111–116.Google Scholar
Kawahara, S., Noto, A., & Kumagai, G. (2018b). Sound symbolic patterns in Pokémon names. Phonetica, 75(3), 481–522. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kawahara, S. & Moore, J. (2021). How to express evolution in English Pokémon names. Linguistics. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kawahara, S., & Shinohara, K. (2012). A tripartite trans-modal relationship between sounds, shapes and emotions: A case of abrupt modulation. The proceedings of the 34th annual meeting of cognitive science society, 569–574.Google Scholar
Kawahara, S., Shinohara, K., & Grady, J. (2015). Iconic inferences about personality: From sounds and shapes. In M. Hiraga, W. Herlofsky, K. Shinohara & K. Akita (Eds.), Iconicity: East meets west (pp. 57–69). Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Kawahara, S., Shinohara, K., & Uchimoto, Y. (2008). A positional effect in sound symbolism: An experimental study. Proceedings of the Japan Cognitive Linguistics association, 81, 417–427.Google Scholar
Köhler, W. (1947). Gestalt psychology: An introduction to new concepts in modern psychology. New York: Liveright.Google Scholar
Kubozono, H. (1999). Nihongo-no onsei: Gendai gengogaku nyuumon 2 [Japanese phonetics: An introduction to modern linguistics 2]. Tokyo: Iwanami Shoten. (In Japanese)Google Scholar
Kumagai, G. (2019). A sound-symbolic alternation to express cuteness and the orthographic Lyman’s Law in Japanese. Journal of Japanese Linguistics, 35(1). 39–74. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kumagai, G., & Kawahara, S. (2019). Effects of vowels and voiced obstruents on Pokémon names: Experimental and theoretical approaches [in Japanese]. Journal of the Linguistic Society of Japan, 1551. 65–99.Google Scholar
(2020). How abstract is sound symbolism? Labiality and diaper names in Japanese [in Japanese]. Journal of the Linguistic Society of Japan, 1571.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago: Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
(1999). Philosophy in the flesh. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Langacker, R. W. (1987). Foundations of cognitive grammar: Theoretical prerequisites. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
(1991). Foundations of cognitive grammar: Descriptive application. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
(2008). Cognitive grammar: A basic introduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Levinson, S. C. (1997). Language and cognition: The cognitive consequences of spatial description in Guugu Yimithirr. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology, 71, 98–131. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lindauer, S. M. (1990). The meanings of the physiognomic stimuli taketa and maluma . Bulletin of Psychonomic Society, 28(1), 47–50. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lockwood, G., & Dingemanse, M. (2015). Iconicity in the lab: A review of behavioral, developmental, and neuroimaging research into sound-symbolism. Frontiers in Psychology. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lupyan, G., & Winter, B. (2018). Language is more abstract than you think, or, why aren’t languages more iconic? Proceedings of Royal Society B, 3731, 20170137. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
MacNeilage, P. F., Davis, B. L., & Matyear, C. L. (1997). Babbling and first words: Phonetic similarities and differences. Speech Communication, 22(2–3), 269–277. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Maurer, D., Pathman, T., & Mondloch, C. J. (2006). The shape of boubas: Sound-shape correspondences in toddlers and adults. Developmental Science, 91, 316–322. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marks, L. (1978). The unity of the senses: Interrelations among the modalities. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Miyakoda, H., & Oshita, M. (2019). Sound symbolism and its effect in character’s names: A study on consonants. Proceedings of the 19th international congress of phonetic sciences, 2134–2138.Google Scholar
Murdock, G. P. (1959). Cross-language parallels in parental kin terms. Anthropological Linguistics, 11, 1–5.Google Scholar
Newman, S. (1933). Further experiments on phonetic symbolism. American Journal of Psychology 45, 53–75. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nobile, L. (2015). Phonemes as images: An experimental inquiry into shape-sound symbolism applied to the distinctive features of French. In M. Hiraga, W. Herlofsky, K. Shinohara & K. Akita (Eds.), Iconicity: East meets west (pp. 71–91). Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Ohala, J. J. (1983). The origin of sound patterns in vocal tract constraints. In P. MacNeilage (Ed.), The production of speech (pp. 189–216). New York: Springer-Verlag. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1994). The frequency code underlies the sound symbolic use of voice pitch. In L. Hinton, J. Nichols & J. J. Ohala (Eds.), Sound symbolism (pp. 325–347). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ota, M. (2015). L1 phonology: phonological development. In H. Kubozono (Ed.), The handbook of Japanese language and linguistics: Phonetics and phonology (pp. 681–717). Berlin & New York: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Perniss, P., Thompson, R. L., & Vigiliocco, G. (2010). Iconicity as a general property of language: Evidence from spoken and signed languages. Frontiers in Psychology. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Perniss, P., & Vigiliocco, G. (2014). The bridge of iconicity: From a world of experience to the experiment of language. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 3691, 20130300. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Proctor, M. I., Shadle, C. H., & Iskarous, K. (2010). Pharyngeal articulation differences in voiced and voiceless fricatives. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 127(3), 1507–1518. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pukui, M. K., & Elbert, E. M. (1979). Hawaiian grammar. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press.Google Scholar
Ramachandran, V. S., & Hubbard, E. M. (2001). Synesthesia–a window into perception, thought, and language. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 81, 3–34.Google Scholar
Sapir, E. (1929). A study in phonetic symbolism. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 121, 225–239. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Saussure, F. (1916/1972). Course in general linguistics. Peru, Illinois: Open Court Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Shih, S. S., Ackerman, J., Hermalin, N., Inkelas, S., & Kavitskaya, D. (2018). Pokémonikers: A study of sound symbolism and Pokémon names. Proceedings of Linguistic Society of America 2018, 3(42), 1–6. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shih, S. S., Ackerman, J., Hermalin, N., Inkelas, S., Jang, H., Johnson, J., Kavitskaya, D., Kawahara, S., Oh, M., Starr, R. L., & Yu. (2019). Cross-linguistic and language-specific sound symbolism: Pokémonastics. Ms. University of Southern California.Google Scholar
Shih, S., & Rudin, D. (2019). On sound symbolism in baseball player names. Ms. University of Southern California.Google Scholar
Shinohara, K., & Kawahara, S. (2009). Onshoochoo no gengokan hikaku [A cross-linguistic comparison of sound symbolism]. Proceedings of the 26th annual meeting of the Japan cognitive science society, 26(O2–1), 1–4. (In Japanese)Google Scholar
(2013). The sound symbolic nature of Japanese maid names. Proceedings of the 13th annual meeting of the Japanese Cognitive Linguistics association, 131, 183–193.Google Scholar
(2016). A cross-linguistic study of sound symbolism: The images of size. Proceedings of the 36th annual meeting of the Berkeley linguistics society, 396–410.Google Scholar
Sidhu, D., & Pexman, P. M. (2017). Five mechanisms of sound symbolic association. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1–25.Google Scholar
(2019). The sound symbolism of names. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 1–5. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Slater, A. S., & Feinman, S. (1985). Gender and the phonology of North American first names. Sex Roles, 131, 429–440. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Spence, C. (2011). Crossmodal correspondences: A tutorial review. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 731, 971–995. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Suzuki, T. (1962). Oninkookan to igibunka no kankei ni tsuite–iwayuru seidakuon tairitsu-o chuushin toshite [The relation between phonological exchange and sense-differentiation: Focusing on so-called voice opposition]. Gengo Kenkyu [Journal of the Linguistic Society of Japan], 421, 23–30. (In Japanese)Google Scholar
Teshigawara, M. (2003). Voices in Japanese animation (Doctoral dissertation). University of Victoria.Google Scholar
Tessier, A.-M. (2010). Short, but not sweet: Markedness preferences and reversals in English hypocoristics. Talk presented at ACL-CLA.
Tomasello, M. (2009). Why we cooperate. Cambridge: MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Uemura, Y. (1965). Onsei-no hyoushousei-ni tsuite [On the symbolic aspects of sounds]. In Gengo seikatsu, 66–70. Tokyo: Honami Shuppan.Google Scholar
Ultan, R. (1978). Size-sound symbolism. In J. Greenberg (Ed.), Universals of human language II: Phonology (pp. 525–568). Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Westbury, J. R., & Keating, P. (1986). On the naturalness of stop consonant voicing. Journal of Linguistics, 221, 145–166. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wichmann, S., Holman, E. W. & Brown, C. H. (2010). Sound symbolism in basic vocabulary. Entropy, 12(4). 844–858. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Whissell, C. (2001). Cues to referent gender in randomly constructed names. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 931, 856–858. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wright, S., & Hay, J. (2002). Fred and Trema: A phonological conspiracy. In S. Benor, M. Rose, D. Sharma, J. Sweetland & Q. Zhang (Eds.), Gendered practices in language (pp. 175–191). Stanford: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Wright, S., Hay, J., & Tessa, B. (2005). Ladies first? Phonology, frequency, and the naming conspiracy. Linguistics, 43(3), 531–561. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yoshimura, K. (2014). Hajimeteno ninchi gengogaku [An introduction to Cognitive Linguistics]. Tokyo: Kenkyuusha. (In Japanese)Google Scholar
Tools and Corpus (on the Web)
LOGIOS Lexicon Tool [URL] (accessed 2018/01/10)
The Disney Wiki: Characters from Theatrical Animated Features [URL] (accessed 2018/01/10).
The Disney Wiki: List of Disney Villains. [URL] (accessed 2018/01/10).
Cited by (12)

Cited by 12 other publications

Kilpatrick, Alexander, Aleksandra Ćwiek, Eleanor Lewis & Shigeto Kawahara
2023. A cross-linguistic, sound symbolic relationship between labial consonants, voiced plosives, and Pokémon friendship. Frontiers in Psychology 14 DOI logo
Schmitz, Dominic, Defne Cicek, Anh Kim Nguyen & Daniel Rottleb
2023. Cuteness modulates size sound symbolism at its extremes. Open Linguistics 9:1 DOI logo
Kumagai, Gakuji, Ryoko Uno & Kazuko Shinohara
Wang, Xiaoxi
2022. Sound symbolism in Chinese children’s literature. Cognitive Linguistics 33:1  pp. 95 ff. DOI logo
Godoy, Mahayana C., André Lucas Gomes, Gakuji Kumagai & Shigeto Kawahara
2021. Sound symbolism in Brazilian Portuguese Pokémon names: Evidence for cross-linguistic similarities and differences. Journal of Portuguese Linguistics 20:1  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo
Kawahara, Shigeto & Canaan Breiss
2021. Exploring the nature of cumulativity in sound symbolism: Experimental studies of Pokémonastics with English speakers. Laboratory Phonology: Journal of the Association for Laboratory Phonology 12:1 DOI logo
Kawahara, Shigeto, Mahayana C. Godoy & Gakuji Kumagai
2021. English Speakers Can Infer Pokémon Types Based on Sound Symbolism. Frontiers in Psychology 12 DOI logo
Kawahara, Shigeto & Gakuji Kumagai
2021. What voiced obstruents symbolically represent in Japanese: evidence from the Pokémon universe. Journal of Japanese Linguistics 37:1  pp. 3 ff. DOI logo
Kawahara, Shigeto & Jeff Moore
2021. How to express evolution in English Pokémon names. Linguistics 59:3  pp. 577 ff. DOI logo
Murai, Shota, Tamaki Noguchi, Makoto Matsumoto, Sachi Itagaki & Kohta I. Kobayasi
2021. 2021 IEEE 3rd Global Conference on Life Sciences and Technologies (LifeTech),  pp. 71 ff. DOI logo
Kumagai, Gakuji
2020. The pluripotentiality of bilabial consonants: The images of softness and cuteness in Japanese and English. Open Linguistics 6:1  pp. 693 ff. DOI logo
Kumagai, Gakuji
2021. Analysing spells in the Harry Potter series: Sound-symbolic effects of syllable lengths, voiced obstruents and low vowels. Open Linguistics 7:1  pp. 511 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 22 september 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.