This paper reports two case studies of sound symbolism using the naturalistic name corpora of characters from Disney and Pokémon. Building upon previous studies of sound symbolism, we tested two hypotheses: (1) voiced obstruents, which are generally associated with negative images, are favored in villainous characters’ names, while (2) bilabial consonants, which are symbolically associated with cuteness, are disfavored in such names. The results show that these tendencies hold in our corpora, suggesting that a concept that is as complex as “villain” can be signaled via sound symbolism. Theoretical implications for cognitive linguistics based on our results are discussed.
Alexander, R. D. (1987). The biology of moral systems. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.
Auracher, J. (2017). Sound iconicity of abstract concepts: Place of articulation is implicitly associated with abstract concepts of size and social dominance. PlosONE, 12(11), e0187196.
Bergen, B. K. (2004). The psychological reality of phonesthemes. Language, 801, 290–311.
Berlin, B. (1994). Evidence for pervasive synesthetic sound symbolism in ethnozoological nomenclature. In L. Hinton, J. Nicols & J. Ohala (Eds.), Sound symbolism (pp. 76–103). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Berlin, B. (2006). The first congress of ethonozoological nomenclature. Journal of Royal Anthropological Institution, 121, 23–44.
Boehm, C. (2012). Moral origins: The evolution of virtue, altruism, and shame. New York: Basic Books.
Brown, R., & Ford, M. (1961). Address in American English. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 621, 375–385.
Cassidy, K. W., Kelly, M. H., & Sharoni, L. J. (1999). Inferring gender from name phonology. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 1281, 362–381.
Cutler, A., McQueen, J., & Robinson, K. (1990). Elizabeth and John: Sound patterns of men’s and women’s names. Journal of Linguistics, 261, 471–482.
Crisinel, A.-S., & Spence, C. (2009). Implicit association between basic tastes and pitch. Neuroscience Letters 4641, 39–42.
Dingemanse, M., Blasi, D. E., Lupyan, G., Christiansen, M. H., & Monaghan, P. (2015). Arbitrariness, iconicity and systematicity in language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(10), 603–615.
D’Onofrio, A. (2014). Phonetic detail and dimensionality in sound-shape correspondences: Refining the bouba-kiki paradigm. Language and Speech, 571, 367–393.
Fuchs, S., Savin, E., Solt, S., Ebert, C., & Krifca, M. (2019). Antonym adjective pairs and prosodic iconicity: Evidence from letter replications in an English blogger corpus. Linguistic Vanguard, 1–15.
Godoy, M. C., Filho, N. S. S., Souza, J. G. M., Alves, H., & Kawahara, S. (2019). Gotta name them all: an experimental study on the sound symbolism of Pokémon names in Brazilian Portuguese. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research.
Haiman, J. (1980). The iconicity of grammar: Isomorphism and motivation. Language, 56(3), 515–540.
Haiman, J. (Ed.). (1985). Iconicity in syntax. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
Hamano, S. (1986). The sound-symbolic system of Japanese (Doctoral dissertation). University of Florida.
Hayes, B., & Steriade, D. (2004). Introduction: The phonetic bases of phonological markedness. In B. Hayes, R. Kirchner, & D. Steriade (Eds.), Phonetically based phonology (pp. 1–33). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Hinton, L., Nichols, J., & Ohala, J. (2006). Sound symbolism (2nd ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Hockett, C. (1959). Animal “languages” and human language. Human Biology, 311, 32–39.
Iwasaki, N., Vinson, D. P., & Vigiliocco, G. (2007). What do English speakers know about gera-gera and yota-yota? A cross-linguistic investigation of mimetic words for laughing and walking. Japanese Language Education Around the Globe, 171, 53–78.
Jaeger, J. J. (1978). Speech aerodynamics and phonological universals. In J. Jaeger, A. Woodbury, F. Ackerman, C. Chiavello, O. Gensler, J. Kingston & K. Whistler (Eds.), Proceedings of Berkeley linguistic society 4 (pp. 311–325). Berkeley: Berkeley Linguistics Society.
Jakobson, R. (1968). Child language, aphasia and phonological universals. (A. Keiler, Trans.). The Hague: Mouton de Gruyter. (Original work published 1941)
Jakobson, R. (1969). Why “mama” and “papa”? In R. Jakobson (Ed.), Selected writings, vol 1: Phonological studies. The Hague & Paris: Mouton de Gruyter.
Jespersen, O. (1922). Symbolic value of the vowel i. In Phonologica. selected papers in English, French and German (Vol. 11) (pp. 283–303). Copenhagen: Levin & Munksgaard.
Johnson, M. (1987). The body in the mind: The bodily basis of meaning, imagination, and reason. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Jurafsky, D. (2014). The language of food: A linguist reads the menu. New York: W. W. Norton & Company.
Kawahara, S. (2017). Introducing phonetics through sound symbolism. Tokyo: Hitsuji Syobo.
Kawahara, S. (2020). Sound symbolism and theoretical phonology. Language and Linguistic Compass.
Kawahara, S. & Breiss, C. (2020). Exploring the nature of cumulativitity through sound symbolism: Experimental studies of Pokémonastics with English speakers. Manuscript, Keio Unviersity and UCLA.
Kawahara, S., Isobe, M., Kobayashi, Y., Monou, T., & Okabe, R. (2018a). Acquisition of sound symbolic values of vowels and voiced obstruents by Japanese children: Using a Pokémonastics paradigm. Journal of the Phonetic Society of Japan, 221, 122–130.
Kawahara, S., & Kumagai, G. (2019a). Expressing evolution in Pokémon names: Experimental explorations. Journal of Japanese Linguistics, 35(1), 2–38.
Kawahara, S. and Kumagai, G. (2019b). Inferring Pokémon types using sound symbolism: The effects of voicing and labiality. Journal of Phonetic Society of Japan, 23(2), 111–116.
Kawahara, S., Noto, A., & Kumagai, G. (2018b). Sound symbolic patterns in Pokémon names. Phonetica, 75(3), 481–522.
Kawahara, S. & Moore, J. (2021). How to express evolution in English Pokémon names. Linguistics.
Kawahara, S., & Shinohara, K. (2012). A tripartite trans-modal relationship between sounds, shapes and emotions: A case of abrupt modulation. The proceedings of the 34th annual meeting of cognitive science society, 569–574.
Kawahara, S., Shinohara, K., & Grady, J. (2015). Iconic inferences about personality: From sounds and shapes. In M. Hiraga, W. Herlofsky, K. Shinohara & K. Akita (Eds.), Iconicity: East meets west (pp. 57–69). Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
Kawahara, S., Shinohara, K., & Uchimoto, Y. (2008). A positional effect in sound symbolism: An experimental study. Proceedings of the Japan Cognitive Linguistics association, 81, 417–427.
Köhler, W. (1947). Gestalt psychology: An introduction to new concepts in modern psychology. New York: Liveright.
Kubozono, H. (1999). Nihongo-no onsei: Gendai gengogaku nyuumon 2 [Japanese phonetics: An introduction to modern linguistics 2]. Tokyo: Iwanami Shoten. (In Japanese)
Kumagai, G. (2019). A sound-symbolic alternation to express cuteness and the orthographic Lyman’s Law in Japanese. Journal of Japanese Linguistics, 35(1). 39–74.
Kumagai, G., & Kawahara, S. (2019). Effects of vowels and voiced obstruents on Pokémon names: Experimental and theoretical approaches [in Japanese]. Journal of the Linguistic Society of Japan, 1551. 65–99.
Kumagai, G., & Kawahara, S. (2020). How abstract is sound symbolism? Labiality and diaper names in Japanese [in Japanese]. Journal of the Linguistic Society of Japan, 1571.
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago: Chicago University Press.
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the flesh. New York: Basic Books.
Langacker, R. W. (1987). Foundations of cognitive grammar: Theoretical prerequisites. Stanford: Stanford University Press.
Langacker, R. W. (1991). Foundations of cognitive grammar: Descriptive application. Stanford: Stanford University Press.
Langacker, R. W. (2008). Cognitive grammar: A basic introduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Levinson, S. C. (1997). Language and cognition: The cognitive consequences of spatial description in Guugu Yimithirr. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology, 71, 98–131.
Lindauer, S. M. (1990). The meanings of the physiognomic stimuli taketa and maluma. Bulletin of Psychonomic Society, 28(1), 47–50.
Lockwood, G., & Dingemanse, M. (2015). Iconicity in the lab: A review of behavioral, developmental, and neuroimaging research into sound-symbolism. Frontiers in Psychology.
Lupyan, G., & Winter, B. (2018). Language is more abstract than you think, or, why aren’t languages more iconic?Proceedings of Royal Society B, 3731, 20170137.
MacNeilage, P. F., Davis, B. L., & Matyear, C. L. (1997). Babbling and first words: Phonetic similarities and differences. Speech Communication, 22(2–3), 269–277.
Maurer, D., Pathman, T., & Mondloch, C. J. (2006). The shape of boubas: Sound-shape correspondences in toddlers and adults. Developmental Science, 91, 316–322.
Marks, L. (1978). The unity of the senses: Interrelations among the modalities. New York: Academic Press.
Miyakoda, H., & Oshita, M. (2019). Sound symbolism and its effect in character’s names: A study on consonants. Proceedings of the 19th international congress of phonetic sciences, 2134–2138.
Murdock, G. P. (1959). Cross-language parallels in parental kin terms. Anthropological Linguistics, 11, 1–5.
Newman, S. (1933). Further experiments on phonetic symbolism. American Journal of Psychology 45, 53–75.
Nobile, L. (2015). Phonemes as images: An experimental inquiry into shape-sound symbolism applied to the distinctive features of French. In M. Hiraga, W. Herlofsky, K. Shinohara & K. Akita (Eds.), Iconicity: East meets west (pp. 71–91). Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
Ohala, J. J. (1983). The origin of sound patterns in vocal tract constraints. In P. MacNeilage (Ed.), The production of speech (pp. 189–216). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Ohala, J. J. (1994). The frequency code underlies the sound symbolic use of voice pitch. In L. Hinton, J. Nichols & J. J. Ohala (Eds.), Sound symbolism (pp. 325–347). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Ota, M. (2015). L1 phonology: phonological development. In H. Kubozono (Ed.), The handbook of Japanese language and linguistics: Phonetics and phonology (pp. 681–717). Berlin & New York: Mouton de Gruyter.
Perniss, P., Thompson, R. L., & Vigiliocco, G. (2010). Iconicity as a general property of language: Evidence from spoken and signed languages. Frontiers in Psychology.
Perniss, P., & Vigiliocco, G. (2014). The bridge of iconicity: From a world of experience to the experiment of language. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 3691, 20130300.
Proctor, M. I., Shadle, C. H., & Iskarous, K. (2010). Pharyngeal articulation differences in voiced and voiceless fricatives. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 127(3), 1507–1518.
Pukui, M. K., & Elbert, E. M. (1979). Hawaiian grammar. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press.
Ramachandran, V. S., & Hubbard, E. M. (2001). Synesthesia–a window into perception, thought, and language. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 81, 3–34.
Sapir, E. (1929). A study in phonetic symbolism. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 121, 225–239.
Saussure, F. (1916/1972). Course in general linguistics. Peru, Illinois: Open Court Publishing Company.
Shih, S. S., Ackerman, J., Hermalin, N., Inkelas, S., & Kavitskaya, D. (2018). Pokémonikers: A study of sound symbolism and Pokémon names. Proceedings of Linguistic Society of America 2018, 3(42), 1–6.
Shih, S. S., Ackerman, J., Hermalin, N., Inkelas, S., Jang, H., Johnson, J., Kavitskaya, D., Kawahara, S., Oh, M., Starr, R. L., & Yu. (2019). Cross-linguistic and language-specific sound symbolism: Pokémonastics. Ms. University of Southern California.
Shih, S., & Rudin, D. (2019). On sound symbolism in baseball player names. Ms. University of Southern California.
Shinohara, K., & Kawahara, S. (2009). Onshoochoo no gengokan hikaku [A cross-linguistic comparison of sound symbolism]. Proceedings of the 26th annual meeting of the Japan cognitive science society, 26(O2–1), 1–4. (In Japanese)
Shinohara, K., & Kawahara, S. (2013). The sound symbolic nature of Japanese maid names. Proceedings of the 13th annual meeting of the Japanese Cognitive Linguistics association, 131, 183–193.
Shinohara, K., & Kawahara, S. (2016). A cross-linguistic study of sound symbolism: The images of size. Proceedings of the 36th annual meeting of the Berkeley linguistics society, 396–410.
Sidhu, D., & Pexman, P. M. (2017). Five mechanisms of sound symbolic association. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1–25.
Sidhu, D., & Pexman, P. M. (2019). The sound symbolism of names. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 1–5.
Slater, A. S., & Feinman, S. (1985). Gender and the phonology of North American first names. Sex Roles, 131, 429–440.
Spence, C. (2011). Crossmodal correspondences: A tutorial review. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 731, 971–995.
Suzuki, T. (1962). Oninkookan to igibunka no kankei ni tsuite–iwayuru seidakuon tairitsu-o chuushin toshite [The relation between phonological exchange and sense-differentiation: Focusing on so-called voice opposition]. Gengo Kenkyu [Journal of the Linguistic Society of Japan], 421, 23–30. (In Japanese)
Teshigawara, M. (2003). Voices in Japanese animation (Doctoral dissertation). University of Victoria.
Tessier, A.-M. (2010). Short, but not sweet: Markedness preferences and reversals in English hypocoristics. Talk presented at ACL-CLA.
Tomasello, M. (2009). Why we cooperate. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Uemura, Y. (1965). Onsei-no hyoushousei-ni tsuite [On the symbolic aspects of sounds]. In Gengo seikatsu, 66–70. Tokyo: Honami Shuppan.
Ultan, R. (1978). Size-sound symbolism. In J. Greenberg (Ed.), Universals of human language II: Phonology (pp. 525–568). Stanford: Stanford University Press.
Westbury, J. R., & Keating, P. (1986). On the naturalness of stop consonant voicing. Journal of Linguistics, 221, 145–166.
Wichmann, S., Holman, E. W. & Brown, C. H. (2010). Sound symbolism in basic vocabulary. Entropy, 12(4). 844–858.
Whissell, C. (2001). Cues to referent gender in randomly constructed names. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 931, 856–858.
Wright, S., & Hay, J. (2002). Fred and Trema: A phonological conspiracy. In S. Benor, M. Rose, D. Sharma, J. Sweetland & Q. Zhang (Eds.), Gendered practices in language (pp. 175–191). Stanford: CSLI Publications.
Wright, S., Hay, J., & Tessa, B. (2005). Ladies first? Phonology, frequency, and the naming conspiracy. Linguistics, 43(3), 531–561.
Yoshimura, K. (2014). Hajimeteno ninchi gengogaku [An introduction to Cognitive Linguistics]. Tokyo: Kenkyuusha. (In Japanese)
2022. Sound symbolism in Chinese children’s literature. Cognitive Linguistics 33:1 ► pp. 95 ff.
Godoy, Mahayana C., André Lucas Gomes, Gakuji Kumagai & Shigeto Kawahara
2021. Sound symbolism in Brazilian Portuguese Pokémon names: Evidence for cross-linguistic similarities and differences. Journal of Portuguese Linguistics 20:1 ► pp. 1 ff.
Kawahara, Shigeto & Canaan Breiss
2021. Exploring the nature of cumulativity in sound symbolism: Experimental studies of Pokémonastics with English speakers. Laboratory Phonology: Journal of the Association for Laboratory Phonology 12:1
Kawahara, Shigeto, Mahayana C. Godoy & Gakuji Kumagai
2021. English Speakers Can Infer Pokémon Types Based on Sound Symbolism. Frontiers in Psychology 12
Kawahara, Shigeto & Gakuji Kumagai
2021. What voiced obstruents symbolically represent in Japanese: evidence from the Pokémon universe. Journal of Japanese Linguistics 37:1 ► pp. 3 ff.
Kawahara, Shigeto & Jeff Moore
2021. How to express evolution in English Pokémon names. Linguistics 59:3 ► pp. 577 ff.
Murai, Shota, Tamaki Noguchi, Makoto Matsumoto, Sachi Itagaki & Kohta I. Kobayasi
2021. 2021 IEEE 3rd Global Conference on Life Sciences and Technologies (LifeTech), ► pp. 71 ff.
Kumagai, Gakuji
2020. The pluripotentiality of bilabial consonants: The images of softness and cuteness in Japanese and English. Open Linguistics 6:1 ► pp. 693 ff.
Kumagai, Gakuji
2021. Analysing spells in the Harry Potter series: Sound-symbolic effects of syllable lengths, voiced obstruents and low vowels. Open Linguistics 7:1 ► pp. 511 ff.
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 22 september 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.