Article published in:
Review of Cognitive Linguistics
Vol. 19:2 (2021) ► pp. 548562
References

References

Briñol, P., & Petty, R. E.
(2008) Embodied persuasion: Fundamental processes by which bodily responses can impact attitudes. In G. R. Semin & E. R. Smith (Eds.), Embodied grounding: Social, cognitive, affective, and neuroscientific approaches (pp. 184–207). New York: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Duncan, L. A., Schaller, M., & Park, J. H.
(2009) Perceived vulnerability to disease: Development and validation of a 15-item self-report instrument. Personality and Individual Differences, 47, 541–546. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Earp, B. D., Everett, J. A., Madva, E. N., & Hamlin, J. K.
(2014) Out, damned spot: Can the “Macbeth Effect” be replicated? Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 36(1), 91–98. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ford, M. B.
(2021) Social distancing during the COVID-19 pandemic as a predictor of daily psychological, social, and health-related outcomes. The Journal of General Psychology, 148(3), 1–23. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Forest, A. L., Kille, D. R., Wood, J. V., & Stehouwer, L. R.
(2015) Turbulent times, rocky relationships: Relational consequences of experiencing physical instability. Psychological Science, 26(8), 1261–1271. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gao, Y., Sun, F., Jiang, W., Fang, Y., Yue, L., Lin, X., & Li, X.
(2020) Beliefs towards the COVID-19 pandemic among patients with emotional disorders in China. General Psychiatry, 33(3), e100231. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, R. W.
(2005) Embodiment and cognitive science. London: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2017) Metaphor wars. London: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hauser, D. J., & Schwarz, N.
(2020) Situated embodiment: When physical weight does and does not inform judgments of importance. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 1–8. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A.
(2010) The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 61–83. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kalichman, S. C.
(1994) Magic Johnson and public attitudes toward AIDS: A review of empirical findings. AIDS Education and Prevention, 6, 542–557.Google Scholar
Kille, D. R., Forest, A. L., & Wood, J. V.
(2013) Tall, dark, and stable: Embodiment motivates mate selection preferences. Psychological Science, 24(1), 112–114. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kövecses, Z.
(2011) Recent developments in metaphor theory: Are the new views rival ones?. Review of Cognitive Linguistics. Published under the Auspices of the Spanish Cognitive Linguistics Association, 9(1), 11–25. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, G.
(2012) Explaining embodied cognition results. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4(4), 773–785. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M.
(1980) Metaphors we live by. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
(1999) Philosophy in the flesh. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Landau, M. J.
(2016) Conceptual metaphor in social psychology: The poetics of everyday life. New York: Routledge. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
[ p. 561 ]
Landau, M. J., Meier, B. P., & Keefer, L. A.
(2010) A metaphor-enriched social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 136(6), 1045–1067. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Li, H.
(2020) Less is more: Motor fluency impairment and body-specific representation of valence. Review of Cognitive Linguistics. Published under the auspices of the Spanish Cognitive Linguistics Association, 18(2), 519–534. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2021) Follow or not follow?: The relationship between psychological entitlement and compliance with preventive measures to the COVID-19. Personality and Individual Differences, 174, 110678. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Li, H., & Cao, Y.
(2019) Losing your footing, losing your morality: The embodied effect of physical slant on moral judgment. Review of Cognitive Linguistics. Published under the auspices of the Spanish Cognitive Linguistics Association, 17(2), 497–510. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2021a) The bright side of the COVID-19 pandemic: Public coughing weakens the overconfidence bias in non-health domains. Personality and Individual Differences, 178, 110861. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2021b) In times of illness: Covid-19 threat influences temporal focus and implicit space-time mappings. Personality and Individual Differences, 171, 110561. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Li, H., & Shen, S.
(2020) A clearer sense of self: relationship between bilingualism and overconfidence bias. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 1–14. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Li, Y., Johnson, E. J., & Zaval, L.
(2011) Local warming: Daily temperature change influences belief in global warming. Psychological Science, 22(4), 454–459. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lu, J. G., Quoidbach, J., Gino, F., Chakroff, A., Maddux, W. W., & Galinsky, A. D.
(2017) The dark side of going abroad: How broad foreign experiences increase immoral behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 112(1), 1–16. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Meier, B. P., Schnall, S., Schwarz, N., & Bargh, J. A.
(2012) Embodiment in social psychology. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4, 705–716. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Meischke, H., Sellers, D. E., Robbins, M. L., Goff, D. C., Daya, M. R., Meshack, A., & Hand, M. M.
(2000) Factors that influence personal perceptions of the risk of an acute myocardial infarction. Behavioral Medicine, 26, 4–13. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F.
(2008) Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior research methods, 40(3), 879–891. Crossref
Ostarek, M., & Huettig, F.
(2019) Six challenges for embodiment research. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28(6), 593–599. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Prentice, D. A., & Miller, D. T.
(1992) When small effects are impressive. Psychological Bulletin, 112(1), 160–164. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ruiz de Mendoza Ibáñez, F. J., & Pérez-Hernández, L.
(2011) The contemporary theory of metaphor: Myths, developments and challenges. Metaphor and Symbol, 26(3), 161–185. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
[ p. 562 ]
Sheetal, A., Feng, Z., & Savani, K.
(2020) Using machine learning to generate novel hypotheses: Increasing optimism about COVID-19 makes people less willing to justify unethical behaviors. Psychological Science, 31(10), 1222–1235. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Varma, D. D. V. N. A., Bagaria, A., Kumari, S., Rajan, M., Priya, A., & Brahmbhatt, H.
(2020) Knowledge, awareness and practice of safety guidelines among layman population regarding prevention of COVID-19 infection. Journal of Advanced Medical and Dental Sciences Research, 8(12), 35–38.Google Scholar
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A.
(1988) Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063–1070. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Williams, L. E., & Bargh, J. A.
(2008) Experiencing physical warmth promotes interpersonal warmth. Science, 322(5901), 606–607. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Williams, L. E., Huang, J. Y., & Bargh, J. A.
(2009) The scaffolded mind: Higher mental processes are grounded in early experience of the physical world. European Journal of Social Psychology, 39(7), 1257–1267. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Zhong, C. B., & Liljenquist, K.
(2006) Washing away your sins: Threatened morality and physical cleansing. Science, 313(5792), 1451–1452. CrossrefGoogle Scholar