Article published In:
Review of Cognitive Linguistics
Vol. 19:2 (2021) ► pp.548562
References
Briñol, P., & Petty, R. E.
(2008) Embodied persuasion: Fundamental processes by which bodily responses can impact attitudes. In G. R. Semin & E. R. Smith (Eds.), Embodied grounding: Social, cognitive, affective, and neuroscientific approaches (pp. 184–207). New York: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Duncan, L. A., Schaller, M., & Park, J. H.
(2009) Perceived vulnerability to disease: Development and validation of a 15-item self-report instrument. Personality and Individual Differences, 471, 541–546. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Earp, B. D., Everett, J. A., Madva, E. N., & Hamlin, J. K.
(2014) Out, damned spot: Can the “Macbeth Effect” be replicated? Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 36(1), 91–98. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ford, M. B.
(2021) Social distancing during the COVID-19 pandemic as a predictor of daily psychological, social, and health-related outcomes. The Journal of General Psychology, 148(3), 1–23. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Forest, A. L., Kille, D. R., Wood, J. V., & Stehouwer, L. R.
(2015) Turbulent times, rocky relationships: Relational consequences of experiencing physical instability. Psychological Science, 26(8), 1261–1271. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gao, Y., Sun, F., Jiang, W., Fang, Y., Yue, L., Lin, X., & Li, X.
(2020) Beliefs towards the COVID-19 pandemic among patients with emotional disorders in China. General Psychiatry, 33(3), e100231. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, R. W.
(2005) Embodiment and cognitive science. London: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017) Metaphor wars. London: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hauser, D. J., & Schwarz, N.
(2020) Situated embodiment: When physical weight does and does not inform judgments of importance. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 1–8. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A.
(2010) The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 61–83. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kalichman, S. C.
(1994) Magic Johnson and public attitudes toward AIDS: A review of empirical findings. AIDS Education and Prevention, 61, 542–557.Google Scholar
Kille, D. R., Forest, A. L., & Wood, J. V.
(2013) Tall, dark, and stable: Embodiment motivates mate selection preferences. Psychological Science, 24(1), 112–114. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kövecses, Z.
(2011) Recent developments in metaphor theory: Are the new views rival ones?. Review of Cognitive Linguistics. Published under the Auspices of the Spanish Cognitive Linguistics Association, 9(1), 11–25. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, G.
(2012) Explaining embodied cognition results. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4(4), 773–785. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M.
(1980) Metaphors we live by. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
(1999) Philosophy in the flesh. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Landau, M. J.
(2016) Conceptual metaphor in social psychology: The poetics of everyday life. New York: Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Landau, M. J., Meier, B. P., & Keefer, L. A.
(2010) A metaphor-enriched social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 136(6), 1045–1067. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Li, H.
(2020) Less is more: Motor fluency impairment and body-specific representation of valence. Review of Cognitive Linguistics. Published under the auspices of the Spanish Cognitive Linguistics Association, 18(2), 519–534. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2021) Follow or not follow?: The relationship between psychological entitlement and compliance with preventive measures to the COVID-19. Personality and Individual Differences, 1741, 110678. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Li, H., & Cao, Y.
(2019) Losing your footing, losing your morality: The embodied effect of physical slant on moral judgment. Review of Cognitive Linguistics. Published under the auspices of the Spanish Cognitive Linguistics Association, 17(2), 497–510. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2021a) The bright side of the COVID-19 pandemic: Public coughing weakens the overconfidence bias in non-health domains. Personality and Individual Differences, 1781, 110861. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2021b) In times of illness: Covid-19 threat influences temporal focus and implicit space-time mappings. Personality and Individual Differences, 1711, 110561. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Li, H., & Shen, S.
(2020) A clearer sense of self: relationship between bilingualism and overconfidence bias. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 1–14. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Li, Y., Johnson, E. J., & Zaval, L.
(2011) Local warming: Daily temperature change influences belief in global warming. Psychological Science, 22(4), 454–459. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lu, J. G., Quoidbach, J., Gino, F., Chakroff, A., Maddux, W. W., & Galinsky, A. D.
(2017) The dark side of going abroad: How broad foreign experiences increase immoral behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 112(1), 1–16. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meier, B. P., Schnall, S., Schwarz, N., & Bargh, J. A.
(2012) Embodiment in social psychology. Topics in Cognitive Science, 41, 705–716. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meischke, H., Sellers, D. E., Robbins, M. L., Goff, D. C., Daya, M. R., Meshack, A., & Hand, M. M.
(2000) Factors that influence personal perceptions of the risk of an acute myocardial infarction. Behavioral Medicine, 261, 4–13. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F.
(2008) Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior research methods, 40(3), 879–891. DOI logo
Ostarek, M., & Huettig, F.
(2019) Six challenges for embodiment research. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28(6), 593–599. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Prentice, D. A., & Miller, D. T.
(1992) When small effects are impressive. Psychological Bulletin, 112(1), 160–164. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ruiz de Mendoza Ibáñez, F. J., & Pérez-Hernández, L.
(2011) The contemporary theory of metaphor: Myths, developments and challenges. Metaphor and Symbol, 26(3), 161–185. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sheetal, A., Feng, Z., & Savani, K.
(2020) Using machine learning to generate novel hypotheses: Increasing optimism about COVID-19 makes people less willing to justify unethical behaviors. Psychological Science, 31(10), 1222–1235. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Varma, D. D. V. N. A., Bagaria, A., Kumari, S., Rajan, M., Priya, A., & Brahmbhatt, H.
(2020) Knowledge, awareness and practice of safety guidelines among layman population regarding prevention of COVID-19 infection. Journal of Advanced Medical and Dental Sciences Research, 8(12), 35–38.Google Scholar
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A.
(1988) Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063–1070. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Williams, L. E., & Bargh, J. A.
(2008) Experiencing physical warmth promotes interpersonal warmth. Science, 322(5901), 606–607. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Williams, L. E., Huang, J. Y., & Bargh, J. A.
(2009) The scaffolded mind: Higher mental processes are grounded in early experience of the physical world. European Journal of Social Psychology, 39(7), 1257–1267. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zhong, C. B., & Liljenquist, K.
(2006) Washing away your sins: Threatened morality and physical cleansing. Science, 313(5792), 1451–1452. DOI logoGoogle Scholar