References (27)
References
Asif, M., Zhiyong, D., Iram, A., & Nisar, M. (2021). Linguistic analysis of neologism related to coronavirus. Social Sciences and Humanities Open, 4 (1), 100201. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC). (2007). Interim pre-pandemic planning guidance: Community strategy for pandemic influenza mitigation in the United States – Centers early, targeted, layered use of nonpharmaceutical interventions. Retrieved from [URL] (Accessed on March 14 2022).
Charteris-Black, J. (2021). Metaphors of coronavirus: Invisible enemy or zombie apocalypse? Cham: Palgrave Macmillan. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Croft, W. (1993). The role of domains in the interpretation of metaphors and metonymies. Cognitive Linguistics, 4 (4), 335–370. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dancygier, B. (2009). Genitives and proper names in constructional blends. In V. Evans & S. Pourcel (Eds.), New directions in Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 161–181). Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dancygier, B., & Sweetser, E. (2005). Mental spaces in grammar: Conditional constructions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(Eds.). (2014). Figurative language. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Fauconnier, G., & Turner, M. (2002). The way we think: Conceptual blending and the mind’s hidden complexities. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Fillmore, C. J. (1985). Frames and the semantics of understanding. Quaderni di Semantica, 6 (2), 222–254.Google Scholar
(1997). Lectures on deixis. Stanford: CSLI.Google Scholar
(2006[1982]). Frame semantics. In D. Geeraerts (Ed.), Cognitive Linguistics: Basic readings (pp. 373–400). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jenson, H. B. (2020). How did ‘flatten the curve’ become ‘flatten the economy?’: A perspective from the United States of America. Asian Journal of Psychiatry, 51 1, 102165. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, fire, and dangerous things: What categories reveal about the mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2006[1993]). Chapter 6: Conceptual metaphor: The contemporary theory of metaphor. In D. Geeraerts (Ed.), Cognitive Linguistics: Basic readings (pp. 185–238). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
(1999). Philosophy in the flesh: The embodied mind and its challenge to western thought. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G., & Núñez, R. E. (2000). Where mathematics comes from: How the embodied mind brings mathematics into being. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Lei, S., Yang, R., & Huang, C.-R. (2021). Emergent neologism: A study of an emerging meaning with competing forms based on the first six months of COVID-19. Lingua, 258 1, 103095. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Páez, D., & Pérez, J. A. (2020). Social representations of COVID-19. International Journal of Social Psychology, 35 (3), 600–610. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pannain, R., & di Pace, L. (2022). Metonymy and the polysemy of Covid in Italian. Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 20 (1), 231–257. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paradis, C. (2011). Metonymization: A key mechanism in semantic change. In R. Benczes, A. Barcelona & F. J. Ruiz de Mendoza (Eds.), Defining metonymy in Cognitive Linguistics: Toward a consensus view (pp. 61–88). Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Radden, G., & Dirven, R. (2007). Cognitive English grammar. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Radden, G., & Kövecses, Z. (1999). Towards a theory of metonymy. In K.-U. Panther & G. Radden (Eds.), Metonymy in language and thought (pp. 17–60). Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ruiz de Mendoza, F. J. (2000). The role of mappings and domains in understanding metonymy. In A. Barcelona (Ed.), Metaphor and metonymy at the crossroads: A cognitive perspective (pp. 109–132). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
(2011). Metonymy and cognitive operations. In R. Benczes, A. Barcelona & F. J. Ruiz de Mendoza (Eds.), Defining metonymy in cognitive linguistics: Towards a consensus view (pp. 103–123). Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ruiz de Mendoza, F. J., & Diez Velasco, O. J. (2002). Patterns of conceptual interaction. In R. Dirven & R. Pörings (Eds.), Metaphor and metonymy in comparison and contrast (pp. 489–532). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ziem, A. (2014). Frames of understanding in text and discourse. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar