Article published In:
Review of Cognitive Linguistics: Online-First ArticlesEmbodied constructions
The encoding of emotions in Ancient Greek
In Ancient Greek two-place verbs can take three different argument structure constructions, with nominative first arguments and either accusative or genitive or dative second arguments. While the accusative is the default case across all verb classes, the genitive indicates low agentivity of the first participant and low affectedness of the second, and the dative is especially frequent with social interaction verbs. The paper focuses on constructional patterns and construction variation with experiential verbs. Referring to data from experimental psychology, I argue that the distribution of constructions with either accusative or genitive second arguments reflects physical embodiment based on narrow vs. broad attention focus, as also indicated by constructional patterns of perception verbs. Emotion verbs that take the dative indicate potentially interactive situations, including those that involve social judgment, thus conforming to the pattern of interaction verbs and reflecting social embodiment.
Keywords: Ancient Greek, argument structure constructions, emotions, narrow vs. broad attention focus, physical and social embodiment
Article outline
- 1.Introduction
- 2.Emotions in cognitive linguistics
- 3.Some background: Argument structure constructions in Ancient Greek
- 3.1Costruction types and frequency
- 3.2Distribution of constructions across verb classes: Experiential verbs
- 3.3From agentivity and control to sight and attentiveness
- 4.Physical embodiment: The NomAcc construction
- 4.1Wonder
- 4.2Fear
- 4.3Between physical and social embodiment: Shame
- 5.Physical embodiment and the NomGen contruction
- 5.1Love and desire
- 5.2Care and affection
- 6.Social embodiment: The NomDat construction
- 6.1Verbs of emotion with the NomDat construction and animate second arguments
- 6.2Verbs with the NomDat construction and inanimate second arguments
- 7.Between physical and social embodiment
- 7.1The double nature of hate
- 7.2The complex construal of care
- 8.Discussion and conclusions
- Acknowledgements
- Notes
-
References
Published online: 15 November 2024
https://doi.org/10.1075/rcl.00205.lur
https://doi.org/10.1075/rcl.00205.lur
References (73)
Aldai, G., & Wichmann, S. (2018). Statistical observations on hierarchies of transitivity. Folia Linguistica,
52
(2), 249–281.
Amberber, M. (2007). Introduction. The language of memory. In M. Amberber (Ed.), The language of memory in crosslinguistic perspective (pp. 1–12). Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
Andreasen, N. J., & Powers, P. S. (1975). Creativity and psychosis: An examination of conceptual style. Archives of General Psychiatry,
32
(1), 70–73.
Athanasiadou, A., & Tabakowska, E. (Eds.). (1998). Speaking of emotions conceptualisation and expression. Berlin & New York: Mouton de Gruyter.
Blume, K. (1998). A contrastive analysis of interaction verbs with dative complements. Linguistics,
36
(2), 253–280.
Cairns, D. L. (1993). Aidôs: The psychology and ethics of honour and shame in ancient Greek literature. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Conti, L., & Luraghi, S. (2014). The Ancient Greek partitive genitive in typological perspective. In S. Luraghi & T. Huumo (Eds.), Partitive case and related categories (pp. 443–476). Berlin & New York: Mouton de Gruyter.
Croft, W. (1993). Case marking and the semantics of mental verbs. In J. Pustejovsky (Ed.), Semantics and the lexicon (pp. 55–72). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Dennett, D. C. (1987). Cognitive wheels: The frame problem in AI. In Z. Pylyshyn (Ed.), The robot’s dilemma: The frame problem and other problems of holism in artificial intelligence (pp. 129–151). Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing.
Derryberry, D., & D. M. Tucker. (1994). Motivating the focus of attention. In P. M. Niedenthal & S. Kitayama (Eds.), The heart’s eye: Emotional influences in perception and attention (pp.167–196). London: Academic Press.
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1975). Unmasking the face: A guide to recognizing emotions from facial clues. Los altos (CA): Malor Books.
Evans, N., & Wilkins, D. (2000). In the Mind’s Ear: the Semantic Extension of Perception verbs in Australian Languages. Language, 76(3), 546–592.
Faucher, L., & Tappolet, C. (2002). Fear and the focus of attention. Consciousness and Emotion,
3
(2), 105–144.
Fisher, N. (1992). Hybris: a study in the values of honour and shame in ancient Greece. Warminster: Aris and Phillips.
Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Review of General Psychology,
2
(3), 300–319.
(2013). Positive emotions broaden and build. In E. Ashby Plant & P. G. Devine (Eds.), Advances on Experimental Social Psychology,
47
1, 1–53.
Goldberg, A. (1995). Constructions: A construction grammar approach to argument structure. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Gruber, J., Culver, J. L., Johnson, S. L., Nam, J. Y., Keller, K. L., & Ketter, T. A. (2009). Do positive emotions predict symptomatic change in bipolar disorder? Bipolar Disorders,
11
1, 330–336.
Harkins, J., & Wierzbicka, A. (Eds). (2001). Emotions in crosslinguistic perspective. Berlin & New York: Mouton de Gruyter.
Haspelmath, M. (2015a). Transitivity prominence. In A. Malchukov & B. Comrie, (Eds.), Valency classes in the world’s languages: A comparative handbook (Vol. 11, pp. 131–147). Berlin & New York: De Gruyter.
Havers, W. (1911). Untersuchungen zur Kasussyntax der indogermanischen Sprachen. Strassburg: Trübner.
Hopper, P. J., & Thompson, S. A. (1980). Transitivity in grammar and discourse. Language,
56
(2), 251–299.
(2006). The anomaly of the verb “give” explained by its high (formal and semantic) transitivity. Linguistics,
44
(3), 569–612.
Kövecses, Z. (1986). Metaphors of anger, pride, and love: A lexical approach to the structure of concepts. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
(2000). Metaphor and emotion: Language, culture, and body in human feeling. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, fire, and dangerous things. What categories reveal about the mind. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Lavidas, N. (2009). Transitivity alternations in diachrony. Changes in argument structure and voice morphology. Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars Publishing.
Lehmann, Ch., & Shin, Y. M. (2005). The functional domain of concomitance. A typological study of instrumental and comitative relations. In Ch Lehmann (Ed.), Typological studies in participation. Berlin: Akademie Verlag.
Luraghi, S. (1995). The function of verb initial sentences in some ancient Indo-European languages. In M. Noonan & P. Downing (Eds.), Word order in discourse (pp. 355–386). Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
(2003). On the meaning of prepositions and cases. A study of the expression of semantic roles in Ancient Greek. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
(2005). The history of the Greek preposition metá: From polysemy to the creation of homonyms. Glotta,
81
1, 130–159.
(2014). Plotting diachronic semantic maps: the role of metaphors. In S. Luraghi & H. Narrog (Eds.), Perspectives on semantic roles (pp. 99–150). Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
(2023). Beyond affectedness — partitive objects and degrees of agenthood in Ancient Greek. Linguistic Variation,
23
(1), 95–123.
(forthcoming). Voice and transitivity with perception verbs in Ancient Greek. Forthcoming in Indogermanische Forschungen 1301 (2025).
Luraghi, S., & Zanchi, Ch. (2018). Double accusative constructions and ditransitives in Ancient Greek. In A. Korn & A. Malchukov (Eds.), Ditransitive constructions in a cross-linguistic perspective (pp. 13–35). Wiesbaden: Reichert.
Malchukov, A. (2005). Case Pattern Splits, Verb Types and Construction Competition. In M. Amberber & H. de Hoop (Eds.), Competition and variation in natural languages: The case for case (pp. 73–117). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
(2008). Split intransitives, experiencer objects and transimpersonal constructions: (re-) establishing the connection. In M. Donohue & S. Wichmann (Eds.), The typology of semantic alignment (pp. 76–101). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
(2017). Exploring the domain of ditransitive constructions: Ditransitive splits and ditransitive alternations across languages. In L. Hellan, A. Malchukov & M. Cennamo (Eds.), Contrastive studies in verbal valency (pp. 177–219). Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
Malchukov, A., Haspelmath, M., & Comrie, B. (2010). Ditransitive construction: a typological overview. In A. Malchukov, M. Haspelmath & B. Comrie (Eds.), Studies in ditransitive constructions: a comparative handbook (pp. 1–64). Berlin & New York: De Gruyter.
Murphy, A. J., & Dubinsky, S. (2023). Classical Greek object cases: A corpus-driven analysis of their distribution. Journal of Greek Linguistics,
23
(1), 97–126.
Næss, Å. (2007). Prototypical transitivity. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
Napoli, M. (2010). The case for the partitive case: The contribution of Ancient Greek. Transactions of the Philological Society,
108
(1), 15–40.
Niemeyer, S., & Dirven, R. (Eds.). (1997). The language of emotions: Conceptualization, expression, and theoretical foundation. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
Noonan, M. (2007). Complementation. In T. Shopen (Ed.), Language typology and syntactic description (pp. 52–150). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Rohrer, T. (2007). the body in space: Embodiment, experientialism and linguistic conceptualization. In J. Zlatev, T. Ziemke, R. Frank & R. Dirven (Eds.), Body, language and mind (vol. 21, pp. 339–378). Berlin & New York: Mouton de Gruyter.
San Roque, L., Kendrick, K. H., Norcliffe, E., & Majid, A. (2018). Universal meaning extensions of perception verbs are grounded in interaction. Cognitive Linguistics,
29
(3), pp. 371–406.
Sausa, E. (2015). Argument structure construction in Homeric Greek. A study on bivalent verbs. PhD thesis, University of Pavia.
Schwarz-Friesel, M. (2015). Language and emotion: The cognitive linguistic perspective. In U. M. Lüdtke (Ed.), Emotion in language: Theory — research — application (pp. 157–173). Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
Stolz, Th. (1997). Some instruments are really good companions — some are not: On syncretism and the typology of comitatives and instrumentals. Theoretical Linguistics,
23
1, 113–200.
(2001). Comitatives vs. instrumentals vs. agents. In W. Bisang (Ed.), Aspects of typology and universals (pp. 153–174). Berlin: Akademie-Verlag.
Stolz, Th., & Gugeler, T. (2000). Comitative typology: nothing about the ape, but something about king-size samples, the European community, and the little prince. Language Typology and Universals — STUF,
53
1, 53–61.
Stolz, Th., Stroh, C., & Urdze, A. (2013). Comitatives and Instrumentals. In M. Dryer & M. Haspelmath (Eds.), WALS Online (v2020.3) [Data set]. Zenodo. (Available online at [URL], Accessed on 2024-03-17.)
Tsunoda, T. (1981). Split case-marking patterns in verb-types and tense/aspect/mood. Linguistics,
19
1, 389–438.
(2015). The hierarchy of two-place predicates: Its limitations. In A. Malchukov & B. Comrie (Eds.), Valency classes in the world’s languages (pp. 1575–1603). Berlin & New York: Mouton De Gruyter.
Verhoeven, E. (2007). Experiential constructions in Yucatec Maya. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.