Article published In:
Expressing and Describing Surprise
Edited by Agnès Celle and Laure Lansari
[Review of Cognitive Linguistics 13:2] 2015
► pp. 436460
References
Averill, J
(1980) A constructivist view of emotion. In R. Plutchik & H. Kellerman (Eds.), Emotion: Theory, research, and experience (pp. 305–340). New York: Academic Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barrett, L.F
(2006) Valence as a basic building block of emotional life. Journal of Research in Personality, 401, 35–55. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barrett, L.F., Mesquita, B., Ochsner, K.N., & Gross, J.J
(2007) The experience of emotion. Annual Review of Psychology, 581, 373–403. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brosch, T
(2009) Novelty (in appraisal). In D. Sander & K.R. Scherer (Eds.), The Oxford companion to emotion and the affective sciences (pp. 285). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cattell, R.B
(1943) The description of personality: Basic traits resolved into clusters. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 381, 476–506. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1945) The description of personality: Principles and findings in a factor analysis. American Journal of Psychology, 581, 69–90. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cohen, J
(1992) A power primer. Psychological Bulleting, 112(1), 155–159. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cook, V.J
(1992) Evidence for multi-competence. Language Learning, 421, 557–591. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2002) Introduction: Background to the L2 user. In V. Cook (Ed.), Portraits of the L2 user (pp. 1–28). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Darwin, C
(1872) The expression of emotions in man and animals (3rd edition., ed. P. Ekman). New York: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
DeLancey, S
(1997) Mirativity: The grammatical marking of unexpected information. Linguistic Typology, 1(1), 33–52. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012) Still mirative after all these years. Linguistic Typology, 16(3), 529–564.Google Scholar
Ekman, P
(1989) The argument and evidence of universals in facial expressions of emotions. In H. Wagner & A. Manstead (Eds.), Handbook of psychophysiology: The biological psychology of emotions and social processes (pp. 143–164). London: John Wiley.Google Scholar
(1992) An argument for basic emotions. Cognition & Emotion, 61, 169–200. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W.V
(1971) Constants across cultures in face and emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 171, 124–129. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ekman, P., Friesen, W.V., & Ellsworth, P
(1982) What emotion categories or dimensions can observers judge from facial behavior? In P. Ekman (Ed.), Emotion in the human face (pp. 39–55). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ekman, P., Sorenson, E.R., & Friesen, W.V
(1969) Pan-cultural elements in facial displays of emotion. Science, 164(3875), 86–88. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fehr, B., & Russell, J.A
(1984) Concept of emotion viewed from a prototype perspective. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 1131, 464–486. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fontaine, J.R.J
(2013) Dimensional, basic emotion and componential approaches to meaning in psychological emotion research. In J.R.J. Fontaine, K.R. Scherer, & C. Soriano (Eds.), Components of emotional meaning: A sourcebook (pp. 31–45). Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fontaine, J.R.J., Poortinga, Y.H., Setiadi, B., & Markam, S.S
(2002) Cognitive structure of emotion terms in Indonesia and The Netherlands. Cognition & Emotion, 161, 61–86. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fontaine, J.R.J., & Scherer, K.R
(2013) The global meaning structure of the emotion domain: Investigating the complementarity of multiple perspectives on meaning. In J.R.J. Fontaine, K.R. Scherer, & C. Soriano (Eds.), Components of emotional meaning: A sourcebook (pp. 106–125). Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fontaine, J.R.J., Scherer, K.R., Roesch, E.B., & Ellsworth, P
(2007) The world of emotion is not two-dimensional. Psychological Science, 181, 1050–1057. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fontaine, J.R.J., Scherer, K.R. & Soriano, C
(Eds.) (2013a) Components of emotional meaning: A sourcebook. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2013b) The why, the what, and the how of the GRID instrument. In J.R.J. Fontaine, K.R. Scherer, & C. Soriano (Eds.), Components of emotional meaning: A sourcebook (pp. 83–97). Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fontaine, J.R.J., & Veirman, E
(2013) The new novelty dimension: Method artifact or basic dimension in the cognitive structure of the emotion domain? In J.R.J. Fontaine, K.R. Scherer, & C. Soriano (Eds.), Components of emotional meaning: A sourcebook (pp. 233–242). Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fontaine, J.R.J., Veirman, E., & Groenvynck, H
(2013) From meaning to experience: The dimensional structure of emotional experiences. In J.R.J. Fontaine, K.R. Scherer, & C. Soriano (Eds.), Components of emotional meaning: A sourcebook (pp. 243–260). Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Frijda, N
(1987) The emotions: Studies in emotion and social interaction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gehm, T., & Scherer, K.R
(1988) Factors determining the dimensions of subjective emotional space. In K.R. Scherer (Ed.), Facets of emotion: Recent research (pp. 99–114). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Gillioz, C., Fontaine, J.R.J., Scherer. K.R., & Soriano, C
submitted). Mapping emotion terms into affective space: Further evidence for a four-dimensional structure. DOI logo
Izard, C.E
(1971) The face of emotion. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
(1977) Human emotions. New York: Plenum Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jakubicek, M., Kilgarriff, A., Kovar, V., Rychly, P., & Suchomel, V
(2013) The TenTen Corpus Family. Paper presented at 7th International Corpus Linguistics Conference , Lancaster, UK.
Kövecses, Z
Krawczak, K., & Glynn, D
(this volume). Operationalizing mirativity. A usage-based quantitative study of constructional construal in English.
Larsen, R.J., & Diener, E
(1992) Promises and problems with the circumplex model of emotion. Review of Personality and Social Psychology, 131, 25–59.Google Scholar
Lazarus, R.S
(1991) Emotion and adaptation. New York/Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Matthews, S
(1998) Evidentiality and mirativity in Cantonese: wo3, wo4, wo5! Proceedings of the international symposium on Chinese languages and linguistics . Academia Sinica.
Meyer, W.U., Niepel, M., Rudolph, U., & Schützwohl, A
(1991) An experimental analysis of surprise. Cognition and Emotion, 51, 295–311. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meyer, W.U., Reisenzein, R., & Schützwohl, A
(1997) Toward a process analysis of emotions: The case of surprise. Motivation and Emotion, 21(3), 251–574. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Niedenthal, P., Auxiette, C., Nugier, A., Dalle, N., Bonin, P., & Fayol, M
(2004) A prototype analysis of the French category “emotion.” Cognition & Emotion, 181, 289–312. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Noordewier, M.K., & Breugelmans, S
(2013) On the valence of surprise. Cognition and Emotion, 27(7), 1326–1334. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ortony, A., Clore, G.L., & Collins, A
(1988) The cognitive structure of emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Osgood, C.E., May, W.H., & Miron, M.S
(1975) Cross-cultural universals of affective meaning. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
Osgood, C.E., Suci, G.J., & Tannenbaum, P.H
(1957) The measurement of meaning. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
Pavlenko, A
(2005) Emotions and multilingualism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Plutchik, R
(1980) A general psychoevolutionary theory of emotion. In R. Plutchik & H. Kellerman (Eds.), Emotion: Theory, research, and experience Vol. 1, Theories of emotion (pp. 3–33). New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Reisenzein, R
(2000) Exploring the strength of association between the components of emotion syndromes: The case of surprise. Cognition and Emotion, 141, 1–34. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Reisenzein, R., & Meyer, W.U
(2009) Surprise. In D. Sander & K.R. Scherer (Eds.), The Oxford companion to emotion and the affective sciences (pp. 386–387). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rosch, E
(1975) Cognitive representation of semantic categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 104(3), 192–233. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rosch, E., & Mervis, C.B
(1975) Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories. Cognitive Psychology, 7(4), 573–605. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Russell, J.A
(1980) A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 391, 1161–1178. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1991) In defense of a prototype approach to emotion concepts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 601, 37–47. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Russell J.A
(2003) Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychological Review, 1101, 145–72. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Russell, J.A., Lewicka, M., & Niit, T
(1989) A cross-cultural study of a circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 571, 848–856. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Saucier, G., & Goldberg, L.R
(1996) The language of personality: Lexical perspectives on the five-factor model. In J.S. Wiggins (Ed.), The five-factor model of personality: Theoretical perspectives (pp. 21–50). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Scherer, K.R
(1984) On the nature and function of emotion: A component process approach. In K.R. Scherer & P. Ekman (Eds.), Approaches to emotion (pp. 293–317). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
(2005) What are emotions? And how can they be measured? Social Science Information, 441, 695–729. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009) The dynamic architecture of emotion: Evidence for the component process model. Cognition & Emotion, 231, 1307–1351. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Scherer, K.R., Fontaine, J.R.J. & Soriano, C
(2013) CoreGRID and MiniGRID: Development and validation of two short versions of the GRID instrument. In J.R.J. Fontaine, K.R. Scherer, & C. Soriano (Eds.), Components of emotional meaning: A sourcebook (pp. 523–541). Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shaver, P., Schwartz, J., Kirson, D., & O’Connor, C
(1987) Emotion knowledge: Further exploration of a prototype approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 521, 1061–1086. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shaver, P., Wu, S., & Schwartz, J
(1992) Cross-cultural similarities and differences in emotion and its representation. A prototype approach. Review of Personality and Social Psychology, 131, 175–212.Google Scholar
Sokolov, E.N., Spinks, J.A., Näätänen, R., & Lyytinen, H
(2002) The orienting response in information processing. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Soriano, C., Fontaine, J.R.J., & Scherer, K.R, & GRID collaborators
(2013) Cross-cultural data collection with the GRID instrument. In J.R.J. Fontaine, K.R. Scherer, & C. Soriano (Eds.), Components of emotional meaning: A sourcebook (pp. 98–105). Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Thayer, R.E
(1989) The biopsychology of mood and arousal. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Torres Bustamante, T
(2012) Real tense and real aspect in mirativity. Proceedings of SALT, 221, 347–364. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van de Vijver, F.J.R., & Leung, K
(1997) Methods and data analysis for cross-cultural research. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Watson, D., & Tellegen, A
(1985) Toward a consensual structure of mood. Psychological Bulletin, 981, 219–235. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 11 other publications

Alba-Juez, Laura & J. Lachlan Mackenzie
2019. Chapter 1. Emotion processes in discourse. In Emotion in Discourse [Pragmatics & Beyond New Series, 302],  pp. 3 ff. DOI logo
Benítez-Castro, Miguel-Ángel & Encarnación Hidalgo-Tenorio
2019. Chapter 12. Rethinking Martin & White’s affect taxonomy. In Emotion in Discourse [Pragmatics & Beyond New Series, 302],  pp. 301 ff. DOI logo
Glynn, Dylan & Avgustina Biryukova
2022. Death, enemies, and illness: How English and Russian metaphorically conceptualise boredom. Yearbook of the German Cognitive Linguistics Association 10:1  pp. 33 ff. DOI logo
Goutéraux, Pascale
2019. Chapter 6. Surprise in native, bilingual and non-native spontaneous and stimulated recall speech. In Surprise at the Intersection of Phenomenology and Linguistics [Consciousness & Emotion Book Series, 11],  pp. 92 ff. DOI logo
Hu, Guangwei & Lang Chen
2019. “To our great surprise …”: A frame-based analysis of surprise markers in research articles. Journal of Pragmatics 143  pp. 156 ff. DOI logo
Juuse, Liina, Kairi Kreegipuu, Nele Põldver, Annika Kask, Tiit Mogom, Gholamreza Anbarjafari & Jüri Allik
2023. Processing emotions from faces and words measured by event-related brain potentials. Cognition and Emotion 37:5  pp. 959 ff. DOI logo
Loderer, Kristina, Kornelia Gentsch, Melissa C. Duffy, Mingjing Zhu, Xiyao Xie, Jason A. Chavarría, Elisabeth Vogl, Cristina Soriano, Klaus R. Scherer & Reinhard Pekrun
2020. Are concepts of achievement-related emotions universal across cultures? A semantic profiling approach. Cognition and Emotion 34:7  pp. 1480 ff. DOI logo
Noordewier, Marret K., Daan T. Scheepers, John F. Stins & Muriel A. Hagenaars
2021. On the physiology of interruption after unexpectedness. Biological Psychology 165  pp. 108174 ff. DOI logo
Piata, Anna
2020. Stylistic humor across modalities. Internet Pragmatics 3:2  pp. 174 ff. DOI logo
Piata, Anna
2022. Stylistic humor across modalities. In The Pragmatics of Internet Memes [Benjamins Current Topics, 120],  pp. 36 ff. DOI logo
Wierzba, Małgorzata, Monika Riegel, Jan Kocoń, Piotr Miłkowski, Arkadiusz Janz, Katarzyna Klessa, Konrad Juszczyk, Barbara Konat, Damian Grimling, Maciej Piasecki & Artur Marchewka
2021. Emotion norms for 6000 Polish word meanings with a direct mapping to the Polish wordnet. Behavior Research Methods 54:5  pp. 2146 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 13 april 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.