Article published In:
Review of Cognitive Linguistics
Vol. 14:2 (2016) ► pp.385415
References
Bender, A., & Beller, S
(2014) Mapping spatial frames of reference onto time: A review of theoretical accounts and empirical findings. Cognition, 1321, 342–382. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bergen, B.K., & Chan Lau, T.T
(2012) Writing direction affects how people map space onto time. Frontiers in Psychology, 31, 109. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bonato, M., Zorzi, M., & Umiltà, C
(2012) When time is space: Evidence for a mental time line. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 361, 2257–2273. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boroditsky, L
(2001) Does language shape thought?: English and Mandarin speakers’ conceptions of time. Cognitive Psychology, 431, 1–22. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2008) Do English and Mandarin speakers think differently about time? In B.C. Love, K. McRae, & V.M. Sloutsky (Eds.), Proceedings of the 30th annual conference of the cognitive science society (pp. 64–70). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society. Google Scholar
Boroditsky, L., Fuhrman, O., & McCormick, K
(2011) Do English and Mandarin speakers think about time differently? Cognition, 1181, 123–129. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Casasanto, D
(2009) When is a linguistic metaphor a conceptual metaphor? In V. Evans & S. Pourcel (Eds.), New directions in cognitive linguistics (pp. 127–145). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Casasanto, D., & Boroditsky, L
(2008) Time in the mind: Using space to think about time. Cognition, 1061, 579–593. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Casasanto, D., & Bottini, R
(2014) Mirror reading can reverse the flow of time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 1431, 473–479. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chan, T.T., & Bergen, B.K
(2005) Writing direction influences spatial cognition. In B. Bara, L. Barsalou, & M. Bucciarelli (Eds.), Proceedings of the 27th annual conference of the cognitive science society (pp. 412–417). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Chen, J.-Y
(2007) Do Chinese and English speakers think about time differently?: Failure of replicating Boroditsky (2001). Cognition, 1041, 427–436. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chen, J.-Y., & Chuang, C.-Y
(2008) Experience with a computer word-entry method in processing Chinese characters by fluent typists. In B.C. Love, K. McRae, & V.M. Sloutsky (Eds.), Proceedings of the 30th annual conference of the cognitive science society (pp. 487–492). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society. Google Scholar
Chen J.-Y., Friedrich, M., & Shu, H
(2015) The effect of immediate and lifetime experience of reading horizontal and vertical texts on Chinese speakers temporal orientation. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 151, 1–12. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chen, J.-Y., & O’Seaghdha, P.G
(2013) Do Mandarin and English speakers think about time differently?: Review of existing evidence and some new data. Journal of Chinese Linguistics, 411, 338–358.Google Scholar
Cooperrider, K., & Núñez, R
(2009) Transversal temporal gestures. Gesture, 91, 181–206. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crawford, L.E., Regier, T., & Huttenlocher, J
(2000) Linguistic and non-linguistic spatial categorization. Cognition, 751, 209–235. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dehaene, S., Izard, V., Pica, P., & Spelke, E
(2006) Core knowledge of geometry in an Amazonian Indigene group. Science, 3111, 381–384. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
de Sousa, H
(2012) Generational differences in the orientation of time in Cantonese speakers as a function of changes in the direction of Chinese writing. Frontiers in Psychology, 31, 255. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fuhrman, O., & Boroditsky, L
(2010) Cross-cultural differences in mental representations of time: Evidence from an implicit nonlinguistic task. Cognitive Science, 341, 1430–1451. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fuhrman, O., McCormick, K., Chen, E., Jiang, H., Shu, D., Mao, S., & Boroditsky, L
(2011) How linguistic and cultural forces shape conceptions of time: English and Mandarin time in 3D. Cognitive Science, 351, 1305–1328. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
January, D., & Kako, E
(2007) Re-evaluating evidence for the linguistic relativity hypothesis: Response to Boroditsky (2001). Cognition, 1041, 417–426. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jarvis, S
(2012) Conceptual transfer. In P. Robinson (Ed.), Routledge encyclopedia of second language acquisition (pp. 115–117). London/New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Jarvis, S., & Pavlenko, A
(2008) Crosslinguistic influence in language and cognition. New York: Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jin, H., & Huang, X
(2012) A new issue in the study of the spatial metaphor of time: The left-and-right representation of time. Advances in Psychology, 201, 1364–1371. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lai, V.T., & Boroditsky, L
(2013) The immediate and chronic influence of spatio-temporal metaphors on the mental representations of time in English, Mandarin, and Mandarin-English speakers. Frontiers in Psychology, 41, 142. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liu, L., & Zhang, J
(2009) The effects of spatial metaphorical representations of time on cognition. Foreign Language Teaching and Research, 411, 266–271.Google Scholar
Majid, A., Gaby, A., & Boroditsky, L
(2013) Time in terms of space. Frontiers in Psychology, 41, 554. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Miles, L.K., Tan, L., Noble, G.D., Lumsden, J., & Macrae, C.N
(2011) Can a mind have two time lines? Exploring space-time mapping in Mandarin and English speakers. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 181, 598–604. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Núñez, R., & Cooperrider, K
(2013) The tangle of space and time in human cognition. Trends in Cognitive Science, 171, 220–229. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Núñez, R.E., & Sweetser, E
(2006) With the future behind them: Convergent evidence from aymara language and gesture in the crosslinguistic comparison of spatial construals of time. Cognitive Science, 301, 401–450. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ouellet, M., Santiago, J., Funes, M.J., & Lupiáñez, J
(2010) Thinking about the future moves attention to the right. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 361, 17–24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pavlenko, A
(2005) Bilingualism and thought. In J. Kroll & A. De Groot (Eds.), Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches (pp. 433–453). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
(2011) Thinking and speaking in two languages. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2014) The bilingual mind and what it tells us about language and thought. 
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Pinker, S
(1994) The language instinct: How the mind creates language. New York: Harper Collins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sweetser, E
(1990) From etymology to pragmatics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tse, C.S., & Altarriba, J
(2008) Evidence against linguistic relativity in Chinese and English: A case study of spatial and temporal metaphors. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 81, 335–357. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tversky, B., Kugelmass, S., & Winter, A
(1991) Crosscultural and developmental-trends in graphic productions. Cognitive Psychology, 231, 515–557. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vallesi, A., Binns, M.A., & Shallice, T
(2008) An effect of spatial-temporal association of response codes: Understanding the cognitive representations of time. Cognition, 1071, 501–527. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vallesi, A., Weisblatt, Y., Semenza, C., & Shaki, S
(2014) Cultural modulations of space-time compatibility effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 211, 666–669. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Whorf, B
(1956) Language, thought and reality: Selected writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf. Ed. by J.B. Carroll. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Xiao, Y
(2012) Conceptualization of time and its linguistic representation. PhD Dissertation, Xinan University.Google Scholar
Xinhua News Agency, & Chinese Ministry of Education
(2006) Over three billion Chinese speakers are learning English. Retrieved September 5, 2014, from [URL].Google Scholar
Xu, S
(2010) A brief reviw of Boroditsky’s. Does language shape thought? Retrieved November 25, 2013, from [URL].
Yang, W
(2015) The influence of Chinese and English speakers’ temporal thinking patterns on language processing. PhD Dissertation, Beijing Foreign Studies University.Google Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 4 other publications

Gu, Yan
2022. Chapter 10. Time in Chinese hands. In Time Representations in the Perspective of Human Creativity [Human Cognitive Processing, 75],  pp. 209 ff. DOI logo
Yang, Wenxing, Xueqin Feng, Jing’ai Jin, Yuting Liu & Ying Sun
2020. Can mirror reading reverse the flow of time? Evidence from Japanese speakers. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica 33:1 DOI logo
Yang, Wenxing, Yiting Gu, Ying Fang & Ying Sun
2022. Mental Representations of Time in English Monolinguals, Mandarin Monolinguals, and Mandarin–English Bilinguals. Frontiers in Psychology 13 DOI logo
Yang, Wenxing & Ying Sun
2018. Do Writing Directions Influence How People Map Space onto Time?. Swiss Journal of Psychology 77:4  pp. 173 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 13 april 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.