References (57)
References
Alarcón, I. (2011). Spanish gender agreement under complete and incomplete acquisition: Early and late bilinguals’ linguistic behavior within the noun phrase. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14 (3), 332–350. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2020). Early and late bilingual processing of Spanish gender, morphology, and gender congruency. BOREALIS: International Journal of Hispanic Linguistics, 9 (2), 175–208. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Arias-Trejo, N., Falcón, A., & Alva-Canto, E. A. (2013). The gender puzzle: Toddlers’ use of articles to access noun information. Psicológica, 34 1, 1–23.Google Scholar
Andersen, R. (1984). What’s gender good for, anyway? In R. Andersen (Ed.), Second languages: A cross-linguistic perspective (pp. 77–99). Newbury House.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Devescovi, A., Hernández, A. & Pizzamiglio, L. (1996). Gender priming in Italian. Perception and Psychophysics, 58 (7), 992–1004. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bruhn de Garavito, J., & White, L. (2002). The second language acquisition of Spanish DPs: The status of grammatical features. In A. T. Pérez-Leroux & J. M. Liceras (Eds.), The Acquisition of Spanish Morphosyntax: The L1/L2Connection (pp. 153–178). Kluwer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Beatty-Martínez, A. L., & Dussias, P. E. (2019). Revisiting masculine and feminine grammatical gender in Spanish: Linguistic, Psycholinguistic, and Neurolinguistic Evidence. Frontiers in Psychology, 10 1, 751. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Camacho, J., & Kirova, A. (2015). Does agreement affect the syntax of bare nominal subjects in Russian–Spanish bilinguals? In R. Klassen, J. M. Liceras & E. Valenzuela (Eds.), Hispanic Linguistics at the crossroads: Theoretical linguistics, language acquisition and language contact. Proceedings of the Hispanic Linguistic Symposium (pp. 169–190). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1995). The minimalist program. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Corbett, G. G. (1991). Gender. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Corbett, G. (2007). Gender and noun classes. In T. Shopen (Ed.), Language typology and syntactic description: III: Grammatical categories and the lexicon, 2nd Ed (241–279). Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Davies, M. (2016–). Corpus of News on the Web (NOW). [URL]
Diebowski, J. (2021). Gender acquisition in Spanish. Effects of language and age. De Gruyter Mouton. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dussias, P. E., Valdés Kroff, J. R., Guzzardo Tamargo, R. E., & Gerfen, C. (2013). When gender and looking go hand in hand: Grammatical gender processing in L2 Spanish. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 35 1, 353–387. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ellis, C., Conradie, S., & Huddlestone, K. (2012). The acquisition of grammatical gender in L2 German by learners with Afrikaans, English and Italian as their L1s. Stellenbosch Papers in Linguistics, 41 1, 17–27. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fernández Fuertes, R., Álvarez de la Fuente, E., & Mujcinovic, S. (2016). The acquisition of grammatical gender in L1 bilingual Spanish. In A. Alba de la Fuente, E. Valenzuela, & C. Martínez Sanz. (Eds.), Language Acquisition Beyond Parameters. Studies in honour of Juana M. Liceras (pp. 237–279). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fernández Fuertes, R., Gómez Carrero, T., & Martínez, A. (2019). Where the eye takes you: The processing of gender in codeswitching. Revista Electrónica de Lingüística Aplicada, 18 (1), 1–17.Google Scholar
Fernández Fuertes, R., & Liceras, J. M. (2018). Bilingualism as a first language: Language dominance and crosslinguistic influence. In A. Cuza & P. Guijarro-Fuentes (Eds.), Language Acquisition and Contact in the Iberian Peninsula (pp. 159–186). Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fernández-García, M. (1999). Patterns of gender agreement in the speech of second language learners. In J. Gutiérrez-Rexach, & F. Martínez-Gil (Eds.), Advances in Hispanic Linguistics: Papers from the 2ndHispanic Linguistics Symposium (pp. 3–15). Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Foote, R. (2014). Age of acquisition and sensitivity to gender in Spanish word recognition. Language Acquisition, 2 (4), 365–385. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Franceschina, F. (2001). Morphological or syntactic deficits in near-native speakers? An assessment of some current proposals. Second Language Research, 17 (3), 213–247. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
González, P., Mayans, D., & van den Bergh, H. (2019). Nominal agreement in the interlanguage of Dutch L2 learners of Spanish. IRAL: International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching. Advance online publication. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Green, J. N. (1988). Spanish. In M. Harris & N. Vincent (Eds.), The Romance Languages (pp. 79–130). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Grüter, T., Lew-Williams, C., & Fernald, A. (2012). Grammatical gender in L2: A production or a real-time processing problem? Second Language Research, 28 (2), 191–215. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Harris, J. W. (1991). The exponence of gender in Spanish. Linguistic Inquiry, 22 (1), 27–62.Google Scholar
Hawkins, R., & Chan, C. Y.-h. (1997). The partial availability of Universal Grammar in second language acquisition: The Failed Functional Features Hypothesis. Second Language Research, 13 (3), 187–226. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hawkins, R., & Franceschina, F. (2004). Explaining the acquisition and non-acquisition of determiner-noun gender concord in French and Spanish. In P. Prévost & J. Paradis (Eds.), The Acquisition of French in Different Contexts (pp. 175–205). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hernández Pina, F. (1984). Teorías psicosociolingüísticas y su aplicación a la adquisición del español como lengua maternal. Siglo XXI.Google Scholar
Hothorn, T., Bretz, F., & Westfall, P. (2008). Simultaneous interference in general parametric models. Biometrical Journal, 50 1, 346–363. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ionin, T., & Zyzik, E. (2014). Judgment and Interpretation Tasks in Second Language Research. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 34 1, 37–64. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kirova, A. (2016). Lexical and morphological aspects of gender and their effect on the acquisition of gender agreement in second language learners [unpublished doctoral dissertation]. Rutgers University.
Kramer, R. (2015). The morphosyntax of gender. The Oxford Studies in Theoretical Linguistics. Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kupisch, T., Akpinar, D., & Stöhr, A. (2013). Gender assignment and gender agreement in adult bilinguals and second language learners of French. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 3 (2), 150–179. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lawrence, M. A. (2011). ez: Easy analysis and visualization of factorial experiments. R package version 31.0–0. [URL]
Lew-Williams, C. & Fernald, A. (2010). Real-time processing of gender-marked articles by native and non-native Spanish speakers. Journal of Memory and Language, 63 (4), 447–464. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
López Ornat, S. (1997). What lies in between a pre-grammatical and a grammatical representation? Evidence on nominal and verbal form–function mapping in Spanish from 1;7 to 2;1. In A. T. Pérez-Leroux & W. Glass (Eds.), Contemporary Perspectives on the Acquisition of Spanish, Volume I: Developing Grammars (pp. 3–20). Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Mariscal, S. (2009). Early acquisition of gender agreement in the Spanish noun phrase: Starting small. Journal of Child Language, 36 (1), 143–171. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Martoccio, A. (2019). Gender assignment strategies used by L1 and L2 speakers of Spanish. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching. Advance online publication. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McCarthy, C. (2008). Morphological variability in the comprehension of agreement: An argument for representation over computation. Second Language Research, 24 1, 459–486. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meisel, J. M. (2009). Second language acquisition in early childhood. Zeitschrift für Sprachwissenschaft, 28 1, 5–34. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S., Davidson, J., de la Fuente, I., & Foote, R. (2014). Early language experience facilitates the processing of gender agreement in Spanish heritage speakers. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17 (1), 118–138. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S., Foote, R., & Perpiñán, S. (2008). Gender agreement in adult second language learners and adult Spanish heritage speakers: The effects of age and context of acquisition. Language Learning, 58 (3), 503–553. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ogneva, A. (2021). La sensibilidad a las claves lingüísticas y el uso del género gramatical en español: un estudio con niños diagnosticados con Trastorno Evolutivo del Lenguaje. Circulo de Lingüística Aplicada a la Comunicación, 85 1, 73–87. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2022). Exploring production of grammatical gender in L2 Spanish: Is a transparent gender system easy to use for Russian-speaking students? Sintagma, 34 1, 4–101.Google Scholar
Park, H. I., Solon, M., Dehghan-Chaleshtori, M., & Ghanbar, H. (2021). Proficiency reporting practices in research on second language acquisition: Have we made any progress? Language Learning, 72 (1), 198–236. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pérez-Pereira, M. (1991). The acquisition of gender: What Spanish children tell us. Journal of Child Language, 18 (3), 571–590. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Prévost, P., & White, L. (2000). Missing surface inflection or impairment in second language acquisition? Evidence from tense and agreement. Second Language Research, 16 (2), 103–133. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roca, I. (1989). The organization of grammatical gender. Transactions of the Philological Society, 87 1, 1–32. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sagarra, N., & Herschensohn, J. (2012). Processing of gender and number agreement in late Spanish bilinguals. International Journal of Bilingualism, 17 (5), 607–627. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, B., & Sprouse, R. (1996). L2 cognitive states and the full transfer/full access hypothesis. Second Language Research, 12 (1), 40–72. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sekerina, A., Brooks, P. J., & Kempe, V. (2005, March). Gender transparency facilitates noun selection in Russian. [Paper presentation]. The 18th Annual City University of New York Sentence Processing Conference, Tucson, Arizona.
Schmitt, C., & Miller, K. (2010). Using comprehension methods in language acquisition research. In E. Blom & S. Unsworth (Eds), Experimental Methods in Language Acquisition Research (pp. 35–56). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Socarrás, G. M. (2011). First language acquisition in Spanish. A minimalist approach to nominal agreement. Continuum.Google Scholar
Teschner, R. V., & W. M. Russell. (1984). The gender patterns of Spanish nouns: An inverse dictionary-based analysis. Hispanic Linguistics, 1 1, 115–32.Google Scholar
Tsimpli, I. M., & Dimitrakopoulou, M. (2007). The Interpretability hypothesis: Evidence from wh-interrogatives in second language acquisition. Second Language Research, 23 (2), 215–242. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
White, L., Valenzuela, E., Kozlowska–Macgregor, M., & Leung, Y. (2004). Gender and number agreement in nonnative Spanish. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25 (1), 105–133. DOI logoGoogle Scholar