References
Aitchison, J.
(1992) Good birds, better birds and amazing birds: The development of prototypes. In: P. J. L. Arnaud, & H. Béjoint (eds.), Vocabulary and Applied Linguistics. Palgrave Macmillan. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1994) Words in the mind: An introduction to the mental lexicon. Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Ameel, E., Storms, G., Malt, B. C., & Sloman, S. A.
(2005) How bilinguals solve the naming problem. Journal of Memory and Language, 53 1, 60–80. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bartol Hernández, J. A.
(2006) La disponibilidad léxica. Revista Española de Lingüística, 36 1, 379–396.Google Scholar
Beckage, N. M., & Colunga, E.
(2016) Language networks as models of cognition: Understanding cognition through language. In: A. Mehler, A. Lücking, S. Banisch, P. Blanchard, & B. Job (eds), Towards a theoretical framework for analyzing complex linguistic networks. Understanding complex systems (pp. 3–28). Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carcedo González, A.
(2000) La lengua como manifestación de otredad cultural (o convergencia intercultural), Espéculo: Universidad Complutense de Madrid. [URL]
Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F.
(1975) A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82 ( 6 ), 407–428. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Collins, A. M., & Quillian, M. R.
(1969) Retrieval time from semantic memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 8 (2), 240–248. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1970) Does category size affect categorization time? Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 9 (4), 432–438. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cremer, M., Dingshoff, D., De Beer, M., & Schoonen, R.
(2010) Do word associations assess word knowledge? A comparison of L1 and L2, child and adult word associations. International Journal of Bilingualism, 15 ( 2 ), 87–204.Google Scholar
Cuenca, M. J., & Hilferty, J.
(1999) Introducción a la lingüística cognitiva. Ariel.Google Scholar
De Deyne, S., Kenett, Y. N., Anaki, D., Faust, M., & Navarro, D.
(2017) Large-scale network representations of semantics in the mental lexicon. In: M. N. Jones (ed.), Frontiers of cognitive psychology. Big data in cognitive science (pp. 174–202). Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group.Google Scholar
Dubois, D.
(1991) Sémantique et Cognition. CNRS.Google Scholar
Echeverría, M. S., Urzúa, P., & Figueroa, I.
(2005) Dispogen II. Programa computacional para el análisis de la disponibilidad léxica. Universidad de Concepción.Google Scholar
Echeverría, M. S., Vargas, R., Urzúa, P., & Ferreira, R.
(2008) DispoGrafo: una nueva herramienta computacional para el análisis de relaciones semánticas en el léxico disponible. Revista de Lingüística Teórica y Aplicada, 46 (1), 81–91. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ecke, P., & Hall, C. J.
(2014) The parasitic model of L2 and L3 vocabulary: Evidence from naturalistic and experimental studies. Fórum Lingüístico, 11 (3), 360–372. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ferreira, R. A., & Echeverría, M. S.
(2010) Redes semánticas en el léxico disponible de inglés L1 e inglés LE. Onomázein, 21 1, 133–153. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fitzpatrick, T.
(2007) Word association patterns: Unpacking the assumptions. International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 17 (3), 319–331. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Galloso Camacho, M. V.
(2002) El léxico de los estudiantes preuniversitarios en el distrito universitario de Salamanca, Ediciones Universidad de Salamanca.Google Scholar
Gollan, T. H., Montoya, R. I., Fennema-Notestine, C., & Morris, S. K.
(2005) Bilingualism affects picture naming but not picture classification. Memory & Cognition, 33 1, 1220–1234. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goñi, J., Arrondo, G., Sepulcre, J., Martincorena, I., Vélez de Mendizábal, N., Corominas-Murtra, B., Bejarano, B., Ardanza-Trevijano, S., Peraita, H., Wall, D. P., & Villoslada, P.
(2010a) The semantic organization of the animal category: Evidence from semantic verbal fluency and network theory. Cognitive Processing, 12 ( 2 ), 183–196. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goñi, J., Martincorena, I., Corominas-Murtra, B., Arrondo, G., Ardanza-Trevijano, S. & Villoslada, P.
(2010b) Switcher-random-walks: A cognitive-inspired mechanism for network exploration. International Journal of Bifurcation and Chaos, 20 (3), 913–922. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gozalo, P. & Martin, M.
(2008) Pruebas de nivel ELE. Modelos de examen para determinar el nivel de nuevos estudiantes. Madrid: SGEL.Google Scholar
Gudmundson, A.
(2020) The mental lexicon of multilingual adult learners of Italian L3: A study of word association behavior and cross-lingual semantic priming. In: C. Bardel, & L. Sánchez (eds.), Third language acquisition: Age, proficiency and multilingualism (pp. 67–109). Language Science Press.Google Scholar
Izquierdo Gil, M. C.
(2004) La selección del léxico en la enseñanza del español como lengua extranjera. Su aplicación en el nivel elemental en estudiantes francófonos. Servicio de Publicaciones de la Universidad de Valencia.Google Scholar
Jing, L.
(2012) El estudio de disponibilidad léxica de los estudiantes chinos de español como lengua extranjera. MARCOELE, 14 1, 1–14.Google Scholar
Karuza, E. A., Thompson-Schill, S. L., & Bassett, D. S.
(2016) Local patterns to global architectures: Influences of network topology on human learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20 (8), 629–640. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kleiber, G.
(1995) La semántica de los prototipos. Categoría y sentido léxico. Visor.Google Scholar
Kövecses, Z.
(2006) Language, mind and culture: A practical introduction, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kroll, J. F., Stewart, E.
(1994) Category interference in translation and picture naming: Evidence for asymmetric connections between bilingual memory representations. Journal of Memory and Language, 33 1, 149–174. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, G.
(1987) Women, fire, and dangerous things. University Of Chicago Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Libben, G. et al.
(2011) The Mental Lexicon. Words in the mind, words in the brain [online]. Available: [URL]
Lin, P.-J., Schwanenflugel, P. J., & Wisenbaker, J. M.
(1990) Category typicality, cultural familiarity, and the development of category knowledge. Developmental Psychology, 261, 805–813. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
López González, A. M.
(2010) La evaluación del desarrollo de la competencia léxica en L2 por medio de la disponibilidad léxica. redELE: Revista Electrónica de Didáctica ELE, 18 1.Google Scholar
Malt, B. C., Gennari, S., Imai, M.
(2010) Lexicalization patterns and the world-to-words mapping. In: B. C. Malt, & P. Wolff (eds.), Words and the mind: How words capture human experience (pp. 29–57). Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Malt, B. C., Sloman, S. A., Gennari, S., Shi, M., & Wang, Y.
(1999) Knowing versus naming: Similarity and the linguistic categorization of artifacts. Journal of Memory and Language, 40 1, 230–262. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meara, P.
(1982) Word associations in a foreign language: A Report on the Birkbeck Vocabulary Project. Nottingham Linguistic Circular, 11 (2), 29–37.Google Scholar
Núñez Romero, L.
(2008) Presencia de elementos culturales en el léxico disponible en E/LE de una muestra de estudiantes arabófonos. In: J. Villatoro (ed.), Actas del Segundo Congreso Virtual sobre E/LE. La red como espacio de colaboración, vol. I1, 267–279.Google Scholar
Palapanidi, K.
(2019) Manifestaciones de “clusters” y “switches” en el léxico disponible de aprendices griegos de ELE en diferentes niveles lingüísticos. MarcoEle, 28 1.Google Scholar
Peppard, J.
2007Exploring the relationship between word-association and learners’ lexical development. University of Birmingham.Google Scholar
Precosky, K.
(2011) Exploring the mental lexicon using word association tests: How do native and non-native speakers of English arrange words in the mind? (unpublished PhD thesis). University of Birmingham.
Pranoto, B. E., & Afrilita, L. K.
(2018) The organization of words in mental lexicon: Evidence from word association test. TEKNOSASTIK, 16 ( 1 ), 26–33. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rosch, E.
(1978) Principles of categorization. In: E. Rosch, & B. B. Lloyd (eds.), Cognition and categorization (pp. 27–48). Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Roux, P. W.
(2013) Words in the mind: Exploring the relationship between word association and lexical development. Polyglossia, 24 1, 80–91.Google Scholar
Salcedo, P. & del Valle, M.
(2013) Disponibilidad Léxica Matemática en Estudiantes de Enseñanza Media de Concepción, Chile, Atenas, (online), 1 ( 21 ), 1–16. Available: [URL]
Sánchez-Saus Laserna, M.
(2016) Léxico disponible de los estudiantes de español como lengua extranjera en las universidades andaluzas. Universidad de Sevilla.Google Scholar
Schwanenflugel, P. J., & Rey, M.
(1986) The relationship between category typicality and concept familiarity: Evidence from Spanish- and English-speaking monolinguals. Memory & Cognition, 14 1, 150–163. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Séguin, M.
(2015) Exploration of the relationship between word-association and learners’ lexical development with a focus on American L1 and Croatian L2 speakers. ExELL (Explorations in English Language and Linguistics), 3 (2), 80–101. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shivabasappa, P., Peña, E. D., & Bedore, L. M.
(2017) Typicality effect and category structure in Spanish–English bilingual children and adults. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 60 (6), 1577–1589. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Šifrar Kalan, M.
(2014) Disponibilidad léxica en diferentes niveles de español/lengua extranjera. Studia Romanica Posnaniensia, 41 (1), 63–85. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017) La universalidad de los prototipos semánticos en el léxico disponible de español. VERBA HISPANICA XXIV 1, 147–165.Google Scholar
Stamer, M. K., & Vitevitch, M. S.
(2012) Phonological similarity influences word learning in adults learning Spanish as a foreign language. Bilingualism and Cognition, 15 (2), 490–502. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Troyer, A. K., Moscovitch, M., & Winocur, G.
(1997) Clustering and switching as two components of verbal fluency: evidence from younger and older healthy adults. Neuropsychology, 11 ( 1 ),138–146. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vitevich, M.
(2008) What can graph theory tell us about word learning and lexical retrieval? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 511, 408–422. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wierzbicka, A.
(1992) Semantics, culture and cognition: Universal human concepts in culture-specific configurations. Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wolter, B.
(2001) Comparing the L1 and the L2 mental lexicon: A depth of individual word knowledge model. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 23 (1), 41–69. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2002) Assessing proficiency through word associations: Is there still hope? System, 30 ( 3 ), 315–329. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zareva, A.
(2007) Structure of the second language mental lexicon: How does it compare to native speakers’ lexical organization? Second Language Research, 23 (2), 123–153. DOI logoGoogle Scholar