References (73)
References
Allopenna, P. D., Magnuson, J. S., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1998). Tracking the time course of spoken word recognition using eye movements: Evidence for continuous mapping models. Journal of Memory and Language, 38, 419–439. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 390–412. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. H., van Rij, J., De Cat, C., & Wood, S. (2018). Autocorrelated errors in experimental data in the language sciences: Some solutions offered by Generalized Additive Mixed Models. In D. Speelman, K. Heylen, & D. Geeraerts (Eds.), Mixed effects regression models in linguistics (pp. 49–69). Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baker, W., & Smith, L. C. (2010). The impact of L2 dialect on learning French vowels: Native English speakers learning Québécois and European French. The Canadian Modern Language Review, 66(5), 711–738. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bassetti, B., Mairano, P., Masterson, J., & Cerni, T. (2020). Effects of orthographic forms on second language speech production and phonological awareness, with consideration of speak-level predictors. Language Learning, 70(4), 1218–1256. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Best, C. T. (1995). A direct realist view of cross-language speech perception. In W. Strange (Ed.), Speech perception and linguistic experience: Issues in cross-language research (pp. 171–204). York Press.Google Scholar
Best, C. T., & Tyler, M. D. (2007). Nonnative and second-language speech perception. In M. J. Munro & O.-S. Bohn (Eds.), Second language speech learning: The role of language experience in speech perception and production (pp. 13–34). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Birdsong, D., Gertken, L. M., & Amengual, M. (2012). Bilingual Language Profile: An easy-to-use instrument to assess bilingualism. University of Texas at Austin. Retrieved on 29 October 2024 from [URL]
Brosseau-Lapré, F., Rvachew, S., Clayards, M., & Dickson, D. (2011). Stimulus variability and perceptual learning of nonnative vowel categories. Applied Psycholinguistics, 34(3), 419–441. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brysbaert, M. (2013). LexTALE_FR: A fast, free, and efficient test to measure language proficiency in French. Psychologica Belgica, 53, 23–37. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chevrot, J.-P., Chabanal, D., & Dugua, C. (2007). Pour un modèle de l’acquisition des liaisons basé sur l’usage: Trois études de cas. Journal of French Language Studies, 17(01), 103. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chevrot, J.-P., Dugua, C., & Fayol, M. (2005). Liaison et formation des mots français: Un scénario développemental. Langages, 39(158), 38–52. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009). Liaison acquisition, word segmentation and construction in French: A usage-based account. Journal of Child Language, 36(3), 557–596. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Christophe, A., Peperkamp, S., Pallier, C., Block, E., & Mehler, J. (2004). Phonological phrase boundaries constrain lexical access I. Adult data. Journal of Memory and Language, 51, 523–547. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Connell, K., Hüls, S., Martínez-García, M. T., Qin, Z., Shin, S., Yan, H., & Tremblay, A. (2018). English learners’ use of segmental and suprasegmental cues to stress in lexical access: An eye-tracking study. Language Learning, 68, 635–668. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Coughlin, C. E., & Tremblay, A. (2014). Morphological decomposition in native and non-native French speakers. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(03), 524–542. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cutler, A., Weber, A., & Otake, T. (2006). Asymmetric mapping from phonetic to lexical representations in second-language listening. Journal of Phonetics, 34(2), 269–284. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Darcy, I., Dekydtspotter, L., Sprouse, R. A., Glover, J., Kaden, C., McGuire, M., & Scott, J. H. (2012). Direct mapping of acoustics to phonology: On the lexical encoding of front rounded vowels in L1 English- L2 French acquisition. Second Language Research, 28(1), 5–40. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Desmeules-Trudel, F. (2015). Perception of Québec French nasal vowels by Brazilian learners: A pilot study. Langues et Linguistique, 35, 41–54.Google Scholar
Desmeules-Trudel, F., & Joanisse, M. F. (2020). Discrimination of four Canadian-French vowels by native Canadian-English listeners. J Acoust Soc Am, 147(5), EL391. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2022). Learning unfamiliar words and perceiving non-native vowels in a second language: Insights from eye tracking. Acta Psychol (Amst), 226, 103590. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Desmeules-Trudel, F., & Zamuner, T. S. (2021). Spoken word recognition in a second language: The importance of phonetic details. Second Language Research, 39, 333–362. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dufour, S. (2008). Phonological priming in auditory word recognition: When both controlled and automatic processes are responsible for the effects. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 33–41. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dufour, S., Nguyen, N. l., & Frauenfelder, U. H. (2007). The perception of phonemic contrasts in a non-native dialect. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 121(4), EL131. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dupoux, E., Sebastián-Gallés, N., Navarrete, E., & Peperkamp, S. (2008). Persistent stress ‘deafness’: the case of French learners of Spanish. Cognition, 106, 682–706. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Durand, J., & Lyche, C. (2008). French liaison in the light of corpus data. Journal of French Language Studies, 18(01). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Escudero, P., & Boersma, P. (2004). Bridging the gap between L2 speech perception research and phonological theory. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 26(4), 551–585. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Flege, J. E. (1987). The production of “new” and “similar” phones in a foreign language: Evidence for the effect of equivalence classification. Journal of Phonetics, 15, 47–65. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1995). Second language speech learning: Theory, findings, and problems. In W. Strange (Ed.), Speech perception and linguistic experience: Theoretical and methodological issues (pp. 233–273). York Press.Google Scholar
Flege, J. E., & Bohn, O.-S. (2021). The Revised Speech Learning Model (SLM-r). In R. Wayland (Ed.), Second language speech learning: Theoretical and empirical progress (pp. 1–83). Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gaskell, G., Spinelli, E., & Meunier, C. (2002). Perception of resyllabification in French. Memory & Cognition, 30, 798–810. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gottfried, T. L. (1984). Effects of consonant context on the perception of French vowels. Journal of Phonetics, 12, 91–114. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gottfried, T. L., & Beddor, P. S. (1988). Perception of temporal and spectral information in French vowels. Status Report on Speech Research, SR-93/94, 51–66. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Inceoglu, S. (2016). Effects of perceptual training on second language vowel perception and production. Applied Psycholinguistics, 37(5), 1175–1199. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ito, A., & Knoeferle, P. (2023). Analysing data from the psycholinguistic visual-world paradigm: Comparison of different analysis methods. Behavioral Research Methods, 55(7), 3461–3493. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jun, S.-A. (1998). The Accentual Phrase in the Korean prosodic hierarchy. Phonology, 15, 189–226. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2000). K-ToBI (Korean ToBI) labeling conventions. UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics, 99, 149–173.Google Scholar
Jun, S.-A., & Fougeron, C. (2000). A phonological model of French intonation. In A. Botinis (Ed.), Intonation: Analysis, modeling and technology (pp. 209–242). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2002). Realizations of accentual phrase in French intonation. Probus, 14, 147–172. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kamiyama, T., & Vaissière, J. (2009). Perception and production of French close and close-mid rounded vowels by Japanese-speaking learners. Acquisition et Interaction en Langue Étrangère (Aile… Lia) 2, 9–41. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kaushanskaya, M., Blumenfeld, H. K., & Marian, V. (2020). The Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire (LEAP-Q): Ten years later. Biling (Camb Engl), 23(5), 945–950. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kuhl, P. K. (1991). Human adults and human infants show a “perceptual magnet effect” for the prototypes of speech categories, monkeys do not. Perception & Psychophysics, 50, 93–107. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kuhl, P. K., & Iverson, P. (1995). Linguistic experience and the “perceptual magnet effect”. In W. Strange (Ed.), Speech perception and linguistic experience: Issues in cross-language research (pp. 121–154). York Press.Google Scholar
Lemhöfer, K., & Broersma, M. (2012). Introducing LexTALE: A quick and valid Lexical Test for Advanced Learners of English. Behavioral Research Methods, 44, 325–343. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Levy, E. S. (2009a). Language experience and consonantal context effects on perceptual assimilation of French vowels by American-English learners of French. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 125, 1138–1152. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009b). On the assimilation-discrimination relationship in American English adults’ French vowel learning. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 126(5), 2670–2682. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Levy, E. S., & Strange, W. (2008). Perception of French vowels by American English adults with and without French language experience. Journal of Phonetics, 36(1), 141–157. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liberman, A. M., Harris, K. S., Hoffman, H. S., & Griffith, B. C. (1957). The discrimination of speech sounds within and across phoneme boundaries. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54(5), 358–368. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mackay, A., & Gass, S. M. (2005). Second language research: Methodology and design. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Mattys, S. L., White, L., & Melhorn, J. F. (2005). Integration of multiple speech segmentation cues: a hierarchical framework. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134(4), 477–500. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McQueen, J. M., & Viebahn, M. C. (2007). Tracking recognition of spoken words by tracking looks to printed words. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 661–671. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Melnik, G. A., & Peperkamp, S. (2019). Perceptual deletion and asymmetric lexical access in second language learners. J Acoust Soc Am, 145(1), EL13. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mirman, D. (2014). Growth curve analysis and visualization using R. Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
New, B., Pallier, C., Ferrand, L., & Matos, R. (2001). Une base de données lexicales du français contemporain sur internet: LEXIQUE. L’année Psychologique, 101, 447–462. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Salverda, A. P., Brown, M., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2011). A goal-based perspective on eye movements in visual world studies. Acta Psychol (Amst), 137(2), 172–180. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shoemaker, E. M. (2010). The exploitation of fine phonetic detail in the processing of L2 French. In B. Van Patten & J. Jegerski (Eds.), Research in second language processing and parsing (pp. 259–280). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012). Durational cues to word recognition in spoken French. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35(2), 243–273. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shoemaker, E. M., & Birdsong, D. (2008). La résolution de la liaison en français par des locuteurs natifs et non-natifs. Acquisition et Interaction en Langue Étrangère, 27, 43–62. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Spinelli, E., Cutler, A., & McQueen, J. M. (2002). Resolution of liaison for lexical access in French. Revue Française de Linguistique Appliquée, VII, 83–96. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Spinelli, E., McQueen, J. M., & Cutler, A. (2003). Processing resyllabified words in French. Journal of Memory and Language, 48(2), 233–254. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tanenhaus, M. K., Spivey-Knowlton, M. J., Eberhard, K. M., & Sedivy, J. C. (1995). Integration of visual and linguistic information in spoken language comprehension. Science, 268, 1632–1634. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tremblay, A. (2009). Phonetic variability and the variable perception of L2 word stress by French Canadian listeners. International Journal of Bilingualism, 13, 35–62. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011a). Learning to parse liaison-initial words: An eye-tracking study. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14(03), 257–279. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011b). Proficiency assessment standards in second language acquisition research. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 33, 339–372. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tremblay, A., Broersma, M., & Coughlin, C. E. (2018). The functional weight of a prosodic cue in the native language predicts the learning of speech segmentation in a second language. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21(3), 640–652. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tremblay, A., Broersma, M., Coughlin, C. E., & Choi, J. (2016). Effects of the native language on the learning of fundamental frequency in second-language speech segmentation. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 985. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tremblay, A., Coughlin, C. E., Bahler, C., & Gaillard, S. (2012). Differential contribution of prosodic cues in the native and non-native segmentation of French speech. Laboratory Phonology, 3, 385–423. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tremblay, A., Kim, S., Shin, S., & Cho, T. (2020). Re-examining the effect of phonological similarity between the native- and second-language intonational systems in second-language speech segmentation. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 24(2), 401–413. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tremblay, A., & Spinelli, E. (2013). Segmenting liaison-initial words: The role of predictive dependencies. Language and Cognitive Processes, 28, 1093–1113. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2014). English listeners’ use of distributional and acoustic-phonetic cues to liaison in French: Evidence from eye movements. Language and Speech, 57(3), 310–337. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tremblay, A., Spinelli, E., Coughlin, C. E., & Namjoshi, J. (2018). Syntactic cues take precedence over distributional cues in native and non-native speech segmentation. Language and Speech, 61, 615–631. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tyler, M. D., Best, C. T., Faber, A., & Levitt, A. G. (2014). Perceptual assimilation and discrimination of non-native vowel contrasts. Phonetica, 71(1), 4–21. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Welby, P. (2006). French intonational structure: Evidence from tonal alignment. Journal of Phonetics, 34, 343–371. DOI logoGoogle Scholar