References (78)
References
Abrahamsson, N. (2012). Age of onset and nativelike L2 ultimate attainment of morphosyntactic and phonetic intuition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 34(2), 187–214. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Abu-Rabia, S., & Kehat, S. (2004). The critical period for second language pronunciation: is there such a thing? Educational Psychology, 24(1), 77–97. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Asher, J. J., & Garcia, R. (1969). The optimal age to learn a foreign language. Modern Language Journal, 53(5), 334–341. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ayoun, D. (Ed.). (2007). French applied linguistics. John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baker, C. L. (1979). Syntactic theory and the projection problem. Linguistic Inquiry, 10, 533–582.Google Scholar
Benatia, A. (2021). Focus on form. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., & Hakuta, K. (1994). In other words: The science and psychology of second-language acquisition. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Birdsong, D. (2007). Nativelike pronunciation among late learners of French as a second language. In O-S. Bohn & M. J. Munro (Eds.), Language experience in second language speech learning: In honor of James Emil Flege (pp. 99–116). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bongaerts, T. (1999). Ultimate attainment in L2 pronunciation: The case of very advanced late L2 learners. In D. Birdsong (Ed.), Second language learners and the Critical Period Hypothesis (pp. 133–159). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Bongaerts, T., Planken, B., & Schils, E. (1995). Can late learners attain a native accent in a foreign language? A test of the Critical Period Hypothesis. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 19, pp. 447–465. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1981). Lectures on government and binding. Foris. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H., & Felser, C. (2006). Grammatical processing in language learners. Applied Trends in Cognitive Science, 10, 564–570. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cook, V. (1988). Chomsky’s Universal Grammar. Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Coppieters, R. (1987). Competence differences between native and near-native speakers. Language, 63, 544–573. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dekydtspotter, L., Anderson, B., & Sprouse, R. A. (2007). Syntax-semantics in English-French interlanguage: Advancing understanding of second language epistemology. In D. Ayoun (Ed.), French applied linguistics (pp. 75–102). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dekydtspotter, L., & Farmer, K. (2016). On the processing of subject clefts in English-French interlanguage: Parsing to learn and the subject relativizer qui. In P. Guijarro-Fuentes, K. Schmitz, & N. Müller (Eds.), The acquisition of French in multilingual contexts (pp. 66–93). Multilingual Matters. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dekydtspotter, L., Miller, A. K., Iverson, M., Xiong, Y., Swanson, K., & Gilbert, C. (2021). Minimal brain adaptation for representational prioritization in non-native parsing: Evidence from a time-frequency analysis of recursion in Wh-dependencies in French. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 59, 101002. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dekydtspotter, L., Sprouse, R. A., & Thyre, R. (1999/2000). Interpretation of quantification at a distance in English-French interlanguage: Domain specificity and second language acquisition. Language Acquisition, 8, 465–320. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Delais-Roussarie, E., Kupisch, T., Mairano, P., Santiago, F. & Splendido, F. (2018). ProSeg: A comparable corpus of spoken L2 French. Poster presented at EuroSLA, Münster, Germany, 5–8 September.
Dewaele, J.-M. (Ed.). (2005). Focus on French as a foreign language: Multidisciplinary approaches. Multilingual Matters. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Edmonds, A., & Leclercq, P. (Eds.). (2020). Methodological challenges in SLA: A focus on French. Special issue of Journal of French Language Studies, 30(2), 111–238. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Flynn, S. (1987). A parameter-setting model of L2 acquisition: Experimental studies in anaphora. D. Reidel. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Flynn, S., Epstein, S. D., & Martohardjono, G. (1996). Universal Grammar and second language acquisition: The null hypothesis. Behaviorial and Brain Sciences, 19(4), 746–752. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Foucart, A., & Frenck-Mestre, C. (2011). Grammatical gender processing in L2: Electrophysiological evidence of the effect of L1–L2 syntactic similarity. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14, 379–399. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012). Can late L2 learners acquire new grammatical features? Evidence from ERPs and eyetracking. Journal of Memory and Language, 66, 226–248. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gregg, K. R. (2003). SLA theory: Construction and assessment. In C. Doughty & M. H. Long (Eds.), Handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 831–865). Blackwell. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Guijarro-Fuentes, P., Schmitz, K., & Müller, N. (Eds.). (2016). The acquisition of French in multilingual contexts. Multilingual Matters. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Guillemon, D., & Grosjean, F. (2001). The gender marking effect in spoken word recognition: The case of bilinguals. Memory and Cognition, 29, 503–511. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hartshorne, J. K., Tenenbaum, J. B., & Pinker, S. (2018). A critical period for second language acquisition: Evidence from 2/3 million English speakers. Cognition, 177, 263–277. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hawkins, R. (2001). Second language syntax: A generative introduction. Blackwell.Google Scholar
(2019). How second languages are learned: An Introduction. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hawkins, R., & Franceschina, F. (2004). Explaining the acquisition and non-acquisition of determiner-noun gender concord in French and Spanish. In P. Prévost & J. Paradis (Eds.), The acquisition of French in different contexts: Focus on functional categories (pp. 175–205). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hawkins, R., Towell, R., & Bazergui, N. (1993). Universal Grammar and the acquisition of French verb movement by native speakers of English. Second Language Research, 9, 189–233. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Herschensohn, J. (2000). The second time around: Minimalism and L2 acquisition. John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2006). Français langue seconde: From functional categories to functionalist variation. Second Language Research, 22, 1–19. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hopp, H. (2007). Ultimate attainment at the interfaces in second language acquisition: Grammar and processing (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Technische Universität Braunschweig.
Howard, M. (2005). The emergence and use of the plus-que-parfait in advanced French interlanguage. In J.-M. Dewaele (Ed.), Focus on French as a foreign language: Multidisciplinary approaches (pp. 63–87). Multilingual Matters. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hulk, A. (1991). Parameter setting and the acquisition of word order in L2 French. Second Language Research, 7, 1–34. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ionin, T., & Montrul, S. (2023). Second language acquisition: Introducing intervention research. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ioup, G., Boustagui, E., El Tigi, M., & Moselle, M. (1994). Re-examining the critical period hypothesis: A case study of successful adult SLA in a naturalistic environment. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 16, 73–98. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kamerhuber, J., Horvath, J., & Pustka, E. (2020). Lecture, répétition, parole spontanée: L’impact de la tâche sur le comportement du schwa en FLE. Journal of French Language Studies, 30(2), 161–188. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Klein, W., & Perdue, C. (1992). Utterance structure: Developing grammars again. John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Laval, C. (2008). Measuring primary, secondary and cumulative effects of processing instruction in the acquisition of French (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Greenwich.
Laval, C., & Lowe, H. (2020). The use of eye-tracking in experimental approaches in second language acquisition research: The primary effects of Processing Instruction in the acquisition of the French imperfect. Journal of French Language Studies, 30(2), 189–210. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lenneberg, E. H. (1967). Biological foundations of language. John Wiley.Google Scholar
Liceras, J. (1986). Linguistic theory and second language acquisition. Gunter Narr.Google Scholar
Mairano, P., & Santiago, F. (2020). What vocabulary size tells us about pronunciation skills: Issues in assessing L2 learners. Journal of French Language Studies, 30(2), 141–160. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marsden, E., & Kasprowicz, R. (2017). Foreign language educators’ exposure to research: Reported experiences, exposure via citations, and a proposal for action. Modern Language Journal, 101, 613–642. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marsden, E., Morgan-Short, K., Thompson, S., & Abugaber, D. (2018). Replication in second language research: Narrative and systematic reviews and recommendations for the field. Language Learning, 68, 321–391. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Masatoshi, S., & Loewen, S. (2019). Do teachers care about research? The research–pedagogy dialogue. ELT Journal, 73(1), 1–10. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McDonald, J. L. (2006). Beyond the Critical Period: Processing-based explanations for poor grammaticality judgement performance by late second language learners. Journal of Memory and Language. 55, 381–401. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McManus, K., & Marsden, E. (2019). Using explicit instruction about L1 to reduce crosslinguistic effects in L2 grammar learning: Evidence from oral production in L2 French. Modern Language Journal, 103, 459–480. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Medgyes, P. (2017). The (ir)relevance of academic research for the language teacher. ELT Journal 71(4) 491–98. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mitchell, R., & Myles, F. (2004). Second language learning theories (2nd ed.). Hodder Arnold. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mougeon, R., & Dewaele, J.-M. (Eds.). (2004). Variation in second language acquisition. Special issue of International Review of Applied Linguistics, 42(4), 295–402Google Scholar
Moyer, A. (1999). Ultimate attainment in L2 phonology: The critical factors of age, motivation and instruction. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 21(1), 81–108. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Myles, F., & Towell, R. (Eds.). (2004). Special issue on the acquisition of French as a second language. Journal of French Language Studies, 14(3). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Newport, E. L. (1991). Contrasting conceptions of the critical period for language. In S. Carey & R. Gelman (Eds.), The epigenesis of mind: Essays on biology and cognition (pp. 111–130). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Penfield, W., & Roberts, L. (1959). Speech and brain mechanisms. Princeton University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Perdue, C., Deulofeu, J., & Trévise, A. (1992). The acquisition of French. In W. Klein & C. Perdue, Utterance structure (pp. 225–300). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Prévost, P. (2009). The acquisition of French. John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Prévost, P., & Paradis, J. (Eds.). (2004). The acquisition of French in different contexts: Focus on functional categories. John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Prévost, P., & White, L. (2000). Missing surface inflection or impairment in second language acquisition? Evidence from tense and agreement. Second Language Research, 16, 103–134. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rule, S. (2004). French interlanguage oral corpora: Recent developments. Journal of French Language Studies 14, 343–356. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Saddour, I. (2020). Methodological considerations when piloting an interview protocol: The example of Syrian asylum seekers in France. Journal of French Language Studies, 30(2), 211–238. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sato, M., & Loewen, S. (2019). Toward evidence-based second language pedagogy. In M. Sato & S. Loewen (Eds.), Evidence-based second language pedagogy: A collection of instructed second language acquisition studies (pp. 1–23). Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sneed German, E., Herschensohn, J., & Frenck-Mestre, C. (2015). Pronoun processing in Anglophone late L2 learners of French: Behavioral and ERP evidence. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 34, 15–40. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sorace, A. (1993). Unaccusativity and auxiliary selection in nonnative grammars of Italian and French: Asymmetries and predictable indeterminacy. Journal of French Language Studies 3, 71–93. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sun, J. (2014). Is it necessary for Chinese Mandarin Speakers to mark time? Reflections about the use of temporal adverbs with respect to verbal morphology. In M. Watorek, S. Benazzo, & M. Hickmann (Eds.), Comparative perspectives on language acquisition. A tribute to Clive Perdue (pp. 108–132). Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Towell, R., & Hawkins, R. (1994). Approaches to second language acquisition. Multilingual Matters. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Treffers-Daller, J., & Tidball, F. (2016). Can L2 learners learn new ways to conceptualize events? A new approach to restructuring in motion event construal. In P. Guijarro-Fuentes, K. Schmitz, & N. Müller (Eds.), The acquisition of French in multilingual contexts (pp. 145–184). Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
VanPatten, B. (1996). Input Processing and grammar instruction in second language acquisition. Ablex.Google Scholar
Véronique, G. D. (2014). The acquisition of additive scope particles by Moroccan Arabic L1 learners of French. In C. Lindqvist & C. Bardel (Eds.), The acquisition of French as a second language (pp. 119–141). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
White, L. (1989). Universal Grammar and second language acquisition. John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1996). Clitics in L2 French. In H. Clahsen (Ed.), Generative perspectives on language acquisition: Empirical findings, theoretical considerations, crosslinguistic comparisons (pp. 335–368). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2003). Second language acquisition and Universal Grammar. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Young-Scholten, M. (2013). Low educated immigrants and the social relevance of second language acquisition research. Second Language Research, 29(4), 441–454. DOI logoGoogle Scholar