Part of
Language Acquisition Beyond Parameters: Studies in honour of Juana M. Liceras
Edited by Anahí Alba de la Fuente, Elena Valenzuela and Cristina Martínez Sanz
[Studies in Bilingualism 51] 2016
► pp. 1735
References (68)
References
Baker, M. (2008). The macroparameter in a microparametric world. In T. Biberauer (Ed.), The Limits of Syntactic Variation (pp. 351–374). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Belletti, A., Bennati, E., & Sorace, A. (2007). Theoretical and developmental issues in the syntax of subjects: Evidence from near-native Italian. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 25, 657–689. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Belletti, A., & Leonini, C. (2004). Subject inversion in L2 Italian. In S. Foster-Cohen, M. Sharwood Smith, A. Sorace, & M. Ota (Eds.), EUROSLA Yearbook, Vol 4 (pp. 95–118). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Biberauer, T., Holmberg, A, Roberts, I, & Sheehan, M. (2010). Parametric Variation: Null Subjects in Minimalist Theory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Borer, H. (1984). Parametric Syntax. Dordrecht: Foris. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Camacho, J. A. (2013). Null Subjects. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. (1981). Lectures on Government and Binding. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H., & Hong, U. (1995). Agreement and null subjects in German L2 development: new evidence from reaction-time experiments. Second Language Research, 11, 57–87. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Davies, W. (1996). Morphological uniformity and the null subject parameter in adult SLA. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 18, 475–493. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hawkins, R. (2001). Second Language Syntax: A Generative Introduction. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Haznedar, B., & Schwartz, B. D. (1997). Are there optional infinitives in child L2 acquisition? In E. Hughes, M. Hughes, & A. Greenhill (Eds.), Proceedings of the 21st Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 257–268). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Hilles, S. (1986). Interlanguage and the pro-drop parameter. Second Language Research, 2, 33–52. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1991). Access to Universal Grammar in second language acquisition. In L. Eubank (Ed.), Point Counterpoint: Universal Grammar in the Second Language (pp. 305–338). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hoekstra, T., Hyams, N., & Becker, M. (1997). The underspecification of number and the licensing of root infinitives. In E. Hughes, M. Hughes, & A. Greenhill (Eds.), Proceedings of the 21st Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 293–306). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
(1999). The role of the specifier and finiteness in early grammar. In D. Adger, S. Pintzuk, B. Plunkett, & G. Tsoulas (Eds.), Specifiers: Minimalist approaches (pp. 251–270). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Huang, C.-T. J. (1984). On the distribution and reference of empty pronouns. Linguistic Inquiry, 15, 531–574.Google Scholar
(1989). Pro-drop in Chinese: A generalized Control theory. In O. Jaeggli, & K. Safir (Eds.), The Null Subject Parameter (pp. 185–214). Dordrecht: Kluwer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hyams, N. (1986). Language Acquisition and the Theory of Parameters. Dordrecht: Reidel. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hyams, N., & Safir, K. (1991). Evidence, analogy and passive knowledge: Comments on Lakshmanan. In L. Eubank (Ed.), Point Counterpoint: Universal Grammar in the Second Language (pp. 411–418). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ionin, T., & Wexler, K. (2002). Why is ‘is’ easier than ‘-s’? Acquisition of tense/agreement morphology by child second language learners of English. Second Language Research, 18, 95–136. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jaeggli, O. (1982). Topics in Romance Syntax. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar
Jaeggli, O., & Hyams, N. (1988). Morphological uniformity and the setting of the null subject parameter. In J. Blevins & J. Carter (Eds.), Proceedings of NELS 18 (pp. 238–253). Amherst, MA: GLSA.Google Scholar
Jaeggli, O., & Safir, K. (Eds.). (1989). The Null Subject Parameter. Dordrecht: Kluwer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Judy, T., & Rothman, J. (2010). From a superset to a subset grammar and the Semantic Compensation Hypothesis: Subject pronouns and anaphora resolution in L2 English. In K. Franich, K. Iserman, & L. Keil (Eds.), Proceedings of the 34th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 197–208). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Lakshmanan, U. (1991). Morphological uniformity and null subjects in child second language acquisition. In L. Eubank (Ed.), Point Counterpoint: Universal Grammar in the Second Language (pp. 389–410). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1994). Universal Grammar in Child Second Language Acquisition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lardiere, D. (1998). Case and tense in the ‘fossilized’ steady state. Second Language Research, 14, 1–26. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2000). Mapping features to forms in second language acquisition. In J. Archibald (Ed.), Second Language Acquisition and Linguistic Theory (pp. 102–129). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Liceras, J. (1988). Syntax and stylistics: more on the pro-drop parameter. In J. Pankhurst, M. Sharwood Smith, & P. Van Buren (Eds.), Learnability and Second Languages: A Book of Readings (pp. 71–93). Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar
(1989). On some properties of the “pro-drop” parameter: Looking for missing subjects in non-native Spanish. In S. Gass & J. Schachter (Eds.), Linguistic Perspectives on Second Language Acquisition (pp. 109–133). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liceras, J., & Díaz, L. (1998). On the nature of the relationship between morphology and syntax: Inflectional typology, f-features and null/overt pronouns in Spanish interlanguage. In M.-L. Beck (Ed.), Morphology and its Interfaces in Second Language Knowledge (pp. 307–338). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1999). Topic-drop versus pro-drop: Null subjects and pronominal subjects in the Spanish L2 of Chinese, English, French, German, Japanese and Korean speakers. Second Language Research, 15, 1–40. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liceras, J., Díaz, L., & Maxwell, D. (1999). Null subjects in non-native grammars: The Spanish L2 of Chinese, English, French, German, Japanese and Korean speakers. In E. Klein & G. Martohardjono (Eds.), The Development of Second Language Grammars: A Generative Approach (pp. 109–145). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liceras, J., Valenzuela, E., & Díaz, L. (1998). Morphological paradigms and syntax: Null and overt arguments in L1 and L2 acquisition of Spanish. In S. Montrul, & R. Slabakova (Eds.), Proceedings of the 1997 Generative Approaches to Second Language Acquisition Conference (pp. 112–121). Montreal: McGill Working Papers in Linguistics.Google Scholar
Margaza, P., & Bel, A. (2006). Null subjects at the syntax-pragmatics interface: Evidence from Spanish interlanguage of Greek speakers. In M. O’Brien, C. Shea, & J. Archibald (Eds.), Proceedings of the 8th Generative Approaches to Second Language Acquisition Conference (GASLA 2006): The Banff Conference. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
Montalbetti, M. (1984). After Binding: On the Interpretation of Pronouns (Unpublished PhD dissertation). MIT.Google Scholar
Montrul, S., & Louro, C. (2006). Beyond the syntax of the null subject parameter: A look at the discourse-pragmatic distribution of null and overt subjects by L2 learners of Spanish. In V. Torrens & L. Escobar (Eds.), The Acquisition of Syntax in Romance Languages (pp. 401–418). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ortifelli, R., & Grüter, T. (2013). Do null subjects really transfer? In J. Cabrelli Amaro, T. Judy, & D. Pascual y Cabo (Eds.), Proceedings of the 12th Generative Approaches to Second Language Acquisition Conference (GASLA 2013) (pp. 145–154). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
Park, H. (2004). A minimalist approach to null subjects and objects in second language acquisition. Second Language Research, 20, 1–32. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pérez-Leroux, A. T., & Glass, W. (1999). Null anaphora in Spanish second language acquisition: Probabilistic versus generative approaches. Second Language Research, 15, 220–249. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Phinney, M. (1987). The pro-drop parameter in second language acquisition. In T. Roeper, & E. Williams (Eds.), Parameter Setting (pp. 221–238). Dordrecht: Reidel. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Prévost, P., & White, L. (2000). Accounting for morphological variation in L2 acquisition: Truncation or missing inflection? In M. A. Friedemann & L. Rizzi (Eds.), The Acquisition of Syntax: Issues in Comparative Developmental Linguistics (pp. 202–234). New York, NY: Longman.Google Scholar
Rizzi, L. (1982). Issues in Italian Syntax. Dordrecht: Foris. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1986). Null objects in Italian and the theory of pro. Linguistic Inquiry, 17, 501–557.Google Scholar
(1994). Early null subjects and root null subjects. In T. Hoekstra & B. D. Schwartz (Eds.), Language Acquisition Studies in Generative Grammar (pp. 151–176). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roberge, Y. (1990). The Syntactic Recoverability of Null Arguments. Kingston & Montreal: McGill & Queen’s University Press.Google Scholar
Rothman, J. (2009). Pragmatic deficits with syntactic consequences?: L2 pronominal subjects and the syntax–pragmatics interface. Journal of Pragmatics, 41, 951–973. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, B. D., & Sprouse, R. (1996). L2 cognitive states and the full transfer/full access model. Second Language Research, 12, 40–72. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Serratrice, L., Sorace, A., & Paoli, S. (2004). Crosslinguistic influence at the syntax-pragmatics interface: Subjects and objects in English Italian bilingual and monolingual acquisition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 7, 183–205. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sorace, A. (2003). Near-nativeness. In C. Doughty, & M. Long (Eds.), Handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 130–151). Oxford: Blackwell. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011). Pinning down the concept of ‘interface’ in bilingualism. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 1, 1–33. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sorace, A. & Filiaci, F. (2006). Anaphora resolution in near-native speakers of Italian. Second Language Research, 22, 339–368. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Speas, M. (2006). Economy, agreement and the representation of null arguments. In P. Ackema, P. Brandt, M. Schoorlemmer, & F. Werman (Eds.), Arguments and Agreement. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tsimpli, I.-M., & Roussou, A. (1991). Parameter resetting in L2? UCL Working Papers in Linguistics, 3, 149–169.Google Scholar
Tsimpli, I.-M., & Sorace, A. (2006). Differentiating interfaces: L2 performance in syntax-semantics and syntax-discourse phenomena. In D. Bamman, T. Magnitskaia, & C. Zaller (Eds.), Proceedings of the 30th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (BUCLD) (pp. 653–664). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Tsimpli, I., Sorace, A., Heycock, C., & Filiaci, F. (2004). First language attrition and syntactic subjects: A study of Greek and Italian near-native speakers of English. International Journal of Bilingualism, 8, 257–277. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wakabayashi, S. (2002). The acquisition of non-null subjects in English: A minimalist account. Second Language Research, 18, 28–71. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wexler, K. (1994). Optional infinitives, head movement and the economy of derivations. In D. Lightfoot, & N. Hornstein (Eds.), Verb Movement (pp. 305–350). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
White, L. (1985a). The “pro-drop” parameter in adult second language learning. Language Learning, 35, 47–62. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1985b). Universal grammar as a source of explanation in second language acquisition. In B. Wheatley, A. Hastings, F. Eckman, L. Bell, G. Krukar, & R. Rutkowski (Eds.), Current Approaches to Second Language Acquisition: Proceedings of the 1984 University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee Linguistics Symposium (pp. 43–68). Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Linguistics Club.Google Scholar
(1986). Implications of parametric variation for adult second language acquisition: An investigation of the pro-drop parameter. In V. Cook (Ed.), Experimental Approaches to Second Language Learning (pp. 55–72). Oxford: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
(1989). Universal Grammar and Second Language Acquisition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2003). Second Language Acquisition and Universal Grammar. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009). Grammatical theory: interfaces and L2 knowledge. In W. Ritchie, & T. Bhatia (Eds.), The New Handbook of Second Language Acquisition (pp. 49–68). Leeds: Emerald.Google Scholar
(2011). Second language acquisition at the interfaces. Lingua, 121, 577–590. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yuan, B. (1997). Asymmetry of null subjects and null objects in Chinese-speakers’s L2 English. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 19, 467–497. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zobl, H. (1990). Evidence for parameter-sensitive acquisition: A contribution to the domain-specific versus central processes debate. Second Language Research, 6, 39–59. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by (2)

Cited by two other publications

White, Lydia
2021. Universal Grammar. In The TESOL Encyclopedia of English Language Teaching,  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo
VanPatten, Bill
2017. Situating instructed language acquisition. Instructed Second Language Acquisition 1:1  pp. 45 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 25 july 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.