Chapter published in:
Bilingual Cognition and Language: The state of the science across its subfields
Edited by David Miller, Fatih Bayram, Jason Rothman and Ludovica Serratrice
[Studies in Bilingualism 54] 2018
► pp. 81102
References

References

Adi-Japha, E., Berberich-Artzi, & Libnawi, A.
(2010) Cognitive flexibility in drawings of bilingual children. Child Development, 81, 1356–1366. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ågren, M., Granfeldt, J., & Thomas, A.
(2014) Combined effects of age of onset and input on the development of different grammatical structures: A study of simultaneous and successive bilingual acquisition of French. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 4, 462–493. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Akhtar, N., & Menjivar, J. A.
(2012) Cognitive and linguistic correlates of early exposure to more than one language. In J. B. Benson (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior, Vol. 42 (pp. 41–78). Burlington, VT: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Allman, B.
(2005) Vocabulary size and accuracy of monolingual and bilingual preschool children. In J. Cohen, K. T. McAlister, K. Rolstad, & J. MacSwan (Eds.), Proceedings of the 4th International Symposium on Bilingualism (pp. 58–77). Massachusetts, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Andruski, J. E., Casielles, E., & Nathan, G.
(2014) Is bilingual babbling language-specific? Some evidence from a case study of Spanish-English dual acquisition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17, 660–672. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bain, B., & Yu, A.
(1980) Cognitive consequences of raising children bilingually: One parent, one language. Canadian Journal of Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie, 34, 304–313. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Barac, R., Bialystok, E., Castro, D. C., & Sanchez, M.
(2014) The cognitive development of young dual language learners: A critical review. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29, 699–714. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Barbosa, P., Nicoladis, E., & Keith, M.
In press). Bilingual children’s lexical strategies in a narrative task. Journal of Child Language. Crossref
Bartsch, K. & Wellman, H. M.
(1995) Children talk about the mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bedore, L. M., Peña, E. D., Summers, C. L., Boerger, K. M., Resendiz, M. D., Greene, K., Bohman, T. M., & Gillam, R. B.
(2012) The measure matters: Language dominance profiles across measures in Spanish–English bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15, 616–629. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ben-Zeev, S.
(1977) The influence of bilingualism on cognitive strategy and cognitive development. Child Development, 48, 1009–1018. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E.
(2011) Coordination of executive functions in monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110, 461–468. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2009) Bilingualism: The good, the bad, and the indifferent. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12, 3–11. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2001) Bilingualism in development: Language, literacy, and cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Barac, R., Blaye, A., & Poulin-Dubois, D.
(2010) Word mapping and executive functioning in young monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Cognition and Development, 11, 485–508. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Luk, G.
(2007) Lexical access in bilinguals: Effects of vocabulary size and executive control. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 21, 522–538. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., & Luk, G.
(2012) Receptive vocabulary differences in monolingual and bilingual adults. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15, 397–401. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Luk, G., Peets, K. F., & Yang, S.
(2010) Receptive vocabulary differences in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13, 525–531. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., & Martin, M. M.
(2004) Attention and inhibition in bilingual children: Evidence from the dimensional change card sort task. Developmental Science, 7, 325–339. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., & Senman, L.
(2004) Executive processes in appearance–reality tasks: The role of inhibition of attention and symbolic representation. Child Development, 75, 562–579. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bosch, L., & Ramon-Casas, M.
(2014) First translation equivalents in bilingual toddlers’ expressive vocabulary: Does form similarity matter? International Journal of Behavioral Development, 38, 317–322. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bridges, K., & Hoff, E.
(2014) Older sibling influences on the language environment and language development of toddlers in bilingual homes. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35, 225–241. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Byers-Heinlein, K., & Werker, J. F.
(2013) Lexicon structure and the disambiguation of novel words: Evidence from bilingual infants. Cognition, 128, 407–416. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Carlson, S. M., & Meltzoff, A. N.
(2008) Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental Science, 11, 282–298. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cha, K., & Goldenberg, C.
(2015) The complex relationship between bilingual home language input and kindergarten children’s Spanish and English oral proficiencies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107, 935–953. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chondrogianni, V., & Marinis, T.
(2011) Internal and external factors in child second language acquisition. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 1, 318–345. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Comeau, L., Genesee, F., & Lapaquette, L.
(2003) The modeling hypothesis and child bilingual codemixing. International Journal of Bilingualism, 7, 113–126. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Core, C., Hoff, E., Rosario Rumiche, R., & Señor, M.
(2013) Total and conceptual vocabulary in Spanish–English bilinguals from 22 to 30 months: Implications for assessment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56, 1637–1649. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, D., Jergovic, D., Inami, Z., & Theodos, V.
(1997) Monolingual and bilingual children’s use of the mutual exclusivity constraint. Journal of Child Language, 24, 3–24. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
De Houwer, A.
(1990) The acquisition of two languages from birth: A case study. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Deuchar, M., & Quay, S.
(2000) Bilingual acquisition: Theoretical implications of a case study. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dixon, L. Q.
(2011) The role of home and school factors in predicting English vocabulary among bilingual kindergarten children in Singapore. Applied Psycholinguistics, 32, 141–168. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Doyle, A. B., Champagne, M., & Segalowitz, N.
(1978) Some issues in the assessment of linguistic consequences of early bilingualism. In M. Paradis (Ed.), Aspects of bilingualism (pp. 13–20). Columbia, SC: Hornbeam Press.Google Scholar
Duñabeitia, J. A., Hernández, J. A., Antón, E., Macizo, P., Estévez, A., Fuentes, L. J., & Carreiras, M.
(2014) The inhibitory advantage in bilingual children revisited. Experimental Psychology, 61, 234–251. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Engel de Abreu, P. M. J.
(2011) Working memory in multilingual children: Is there a bilingual effect? Memory, 19, 529–537. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fennell, C. T., Tsui, A. S.-M., & Hudon, T. M.
(2016) Speech perception in simultaneously bilingual infants. In E. Nicoladis & S. Montarnari (Eds.), Bilingualism across the lifespan (pp. 43–62). Washington, DC: De Gruyter and American Psychological Association. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, V. C. M., Thomas, E. M., Kennedy, I., Prys, C., Young, N., Vinas Guasch, N., Roberts, E. J., Hughes, E. K., & Jones, L.
(2014) Does language dominance affect cognitive performance in bilinguals? Lifespan evidence from preschoolers through older adults on card sorting, Simon, and metalinguistic tasks. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 11. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Genesee, F.
(2016) Shifting perspectives on bilingualism. In E. Nicoladis & S. Montarnari (Eds.), Bilingualism across the lifespan (pp. 9–19). Washington, DC: De Gruyter and American Psychological Association. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1989) Early bilingual development: One language or two? Journal of Child Language, 16, 161–179. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Genesee, F., Nicoladis, E., & Paradis, J.
(1995) Language differentiation in early bilingual development. Journal of Child Language, 22, 611–631. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Goetz, P. J.
(2003) The effects of bilingualism on theory of mind development. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 6, 1–15. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T. H., Starr, J., & Ferreira, V. S.
(2015) More than use it or lose it: The number-of-speakers effect on heritage language proficiency. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22, 147–155. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Goodz, N. S.
(1989) Parental language mixing in bilingual families. Infant Mental Health Journal, 10, 25–44. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Grosjean, F.
(2001) The bilingual’s language modes. In J. L. Nicol (Ed.), One mind, two languages: Bilingual language processing (pp. 1–20). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Hoff, E., Rumiche, R., Burridge, A., Ribot, K. M., & Welsh, S. N.
(2014) Expressive vocabulary development in children from bilingual and monolingual homes: A longitudinal study from two to four years. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29, 433–444. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Holowka, S., Brosseau‐Lapré, F., & Petitto, L. A.
(2002) Semantic and conceptual knowledge underlying bilingual babies’ first signs and words. Language Learning, 52, 205–262. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hopp, H., & Schmid, M. S.
(2013) Perceived foreign accent in first language attrition and second language acquisition: The impact of age of acquisition and bilingualism. Applied Psycholinguistics, 34, 361–394. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Müller, N., & Hulk, A.
(2001) Crosslinguistic influence in bilingual language acquisition: Italian and French as recipient languages. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 4, 1–21. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hurtado, N., Grueter, T., Marchman, V. A., & Fernald, A.
(2014) Relative language exposure, processing efficiency and vocabulary in Spanish–English bilingual toddlers. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17, 189–202. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hutchison, S. M.
(2010) Executive function and bilingualism: What are the effects of language proficiency. Unpublished MA thesis, University of British Columbia. http://​hdl​.handle​.net​/1828​/3177
Jean-Louis, B.
(1999) Does childhood bilingualism facilitate the development of theory of mind? Unpublished PhD dissertation, Yale University.Google Scholar
Jia, G., Chen, J., Kim, HY, Chan, P-S, & Jeung, C.
(2014) Bilingual lexical skills of school-age children with Chinese and Korean heritage languages in the United States. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 38, 350–358. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Johnstone, N.
(2004) Babbling in bilingual infants: Is there evidence of code switching? Presented at The First International Symposium on Bilingualism and Bilingual Education in Latin America: English Speaking Scholastic Association of the River Plate and Universidad San Andrés, Buenos Aires, April 2004.
Junker, D. A., & Stockman, I. J.
(2002) Expressive vocabulary of German-English bilingual toddlers. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 11, 381–394. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kandhadai, P., Danielson, D., & Werker, J.
(2014) Culture as a binder for bilingual acquisition. Trends in Neuroscience and Education, 3, 24–27. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kupisch, T.
(2007) Determiners in bilingual German-Italian children: What they tell us about the relation between language influence and language dominance. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 10: 57–78. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kupisch, T., Barton, D., Hailer, K., Lein, T., Stangen, I. & van de Weijer, J.
(2014) Foreign accent in adult simultaneous bilinguals? Heritage Language Journal, 11, 123–50.
Google Scholar
Lanvers, U.
(2001) Language alternation in infant bilinguals: A developmental approach to codeswitching. International Journal of Bilingualism, 5, 437–464. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Leopold, W.
(1949) Speech development of a bilingual child (Vol. 4). Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.Google Scholar
Lleó, C., I. Kuchenbrandt, M. Kehoe & C. Trujillo
(2003) Syllable final consonants in Spanish and German monolingual and bilingual acquisition. In N. Müller (ed.), (Non)vulnerable domains in bilingualism (pp. 191–202). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Long, M. H.
(1990) Maturational constraints on language development. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 12, 251–285. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Maneva, B., & Genesee, F.
(2002) Bilingual babbling: Evidence for language differentiation in dual language acquisition. In B. Skarabela, S. Fish, & A. H.-J. Do (Eds.), Proceedings of the 26th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 383–392). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
MacNeilage, P. F.
(Ed.) (1983) The production of speech. Berlin: Springer. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
MacSwan, J.
(2016) Code-switching in adulthood. In E. Nicoladis & S. Montarnari (Eds.), Bilingualism across the lifespan (pp. 183–200). Washington, DC: De Gruyter and American Psychological Association. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mancilla-Martinez, J., & Vagh, S. B.
(2013) Growth in toddlers’ Spanish, English, and conceptual vocabulary knowledge. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 28, 555–567. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Marinova-Todd, S. H.
(2012) “Corplum is a core from a plum”: The advantage of bilingual children in the analysis of word meaning. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15, 117–127. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Meisel, J. M.
(2004) The bilingual child. In T. K. Bhatia & W. C. Ritchie (Eds.), The handbook of bilingualism (pp. 91–113). Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Meisel, J.
(1989) Early differentiation of languages in bilingual children. In K. Hyltenstam & L. Obler (Eds.), Bilingualism across the lifespan: Aspects of acquisition, maturity, and loss (pp. 13–40). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Montarnari, S., & Nicoladis, E.
(2016) Concluding remarks and future directions. In E. Nicoladis & S. Montarnari (Eds.), Bilingualism across the lifespan (pp. 325–330). Washington, DC: De Gruyter and American Psychological Association. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Morales, J., Calvo, A., & Bialystok, E.
(2013) Working memory development in monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114, 187–202. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Morton, J. B., & Harper, S. N.
(2007) What did Simon say? Revisiting the bilingual advantage. Developmental Science, 10, 719–726. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Munro, M., & Mann, V.
(2005) Age of immersion as a predictor of foreign accent. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26, 311–341. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nagpal, J., & Nicoladis, E.
(2010) Positive attitudes are not enough: Minority language survival in the Canadian Prairies. Journal of Intercultural Communication, 24. http://​www​.immi​.se​/jicc​/index​.php​/jicc​/article​/view​/230​/161
Nicoladis, E.
(2016) Measuring language dominance in bilingual children: Ramifications on predicting crosslinguistic influence. In C. Silva-Corvalán & J. Treffers-Daller (Eds.) Operationalising and measuring language dominance (pp. 219–234). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2012) Cross-linguistic influence in French-English bilingual children’s possessive constructions. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15, 320–328. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2006) Cross-linguistic transfer in adjective-noun strings by preschool bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 9, 15–32. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2003) Cross-linguistic transfer in deverbal compounds of preschool bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 6, 17–31. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1998) First clues to the existence of two input languages: Pragmatic and lexical differentiation in a bilingual child. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1, 105–116. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nicoladis, E., & Gavrila, A.
(2015) Cross-linguistic influence in Welsh-English bilingual children’s adjectival constructions. Journal of Child Language, 42, 903–916. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nicoladis, E., & Genesee, F.
(1996) A longitudinal study of language differentiation in young bilingual children. Language Learning, 46, 439–464. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nicoladis, E., Palmer, A., & Marentette, P.
(2007) The role of type and token frequency in using past tense morphemes correctly. Developmental Science, 10, 237–254. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nicoladis, E., Pika, S., Yin, H, & Marentette, P.
(2007) Gesture use in story recall by Chinese-English bilinguals. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 721–735. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nicoladis, E., & Secco, G.
(2000) The role of a child’s productive vocabulary in the language choice of a bilingual family. First Language, 20, 3–28. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Oller, D. K.
(2005) The distributed characteristic in bilingual learning. In J. Cohen, K. T. McAlister, K. Rolstad, & J. MacSwan (Eds.), ISB4: Proceedings of the 4th International Symposium on Bilingualism (pp. 1744–1749). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Oller, D. K., Pearson, B. Z., & Cobo-Lewis, A. B.
(2007) Profile effects in early bilingual language and literacy. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 191–230. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J.
(2001) Do bilingual two-year-olds have separate phonological systems? International Journal of Bilingualism, 5, 19–38. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J., & Genesee, F.
(1996) Syntactic acquisition in bilingual children: Autonomous or interdependent? Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 18, 1–25. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J., & Nicoladis, E.
(2007) The influence of dominance and sociolinguistic context on bilingual preschoolers’ language choice. The International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 10, 277–297. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pearson, B. Z., Fernández, S. C., Lewedag, V., & Oller, D. K.
(1997) The relation of input factors to lexical learning by bilingual infants (ages 10 to 30 months). Applied Psycholinguistics, 18, 41–58. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pearson, B. Z., Fernandez, S. C., & Oller, D. K.
(1993) Lexical development in bilingual infants and toddlers: Comparison to monolingual norms. Language Learning, 43, 93–120. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Perani, D., Paulesu, E., Galles, N. S., Dupoux, E., Dehaene, S., Bettinardi, V., Cappa, S. F., Fazio, F., & Mehler, J.
(1998) The bilingual brain. Proficiency and age of acquisition of the second language. Brain, 121, 1841–1852. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Place, S., & Hoff, E.
(2011) Properties of dual language exposure that influence 2‐year‐olds’ bilingual proficiency. Child Development, 82, 1834–1849. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Poulin-Dubois, D., & Goodz, N.
(2001) Language differentiation in bilingual infants: Evidence from babbling. In J. Cenoz & F. Genesee (Eds.), Trends in bilingual acquisition (pp. 95–106). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rebelos, M.
(2013) ‘Mami, lietadlo! Aeroplane, daddy!’ A case study exploring bilingual first language acquisition in a mixed-lingual family. Unpublished PhD dissertation, University of Adelaide.Google Scholar
Quay, S., & Montarnari, S.
(2016) Early bilingualism: From differentiation to the impact of family language practices. In E. Nicoladis & S. Montarnari (Eds.), Bilingualism across the lifespan (pp. 23–42). Washington, DC: De Gruyter and American Psychological Association. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rodina, Y., & M. Westergaard
(2015) Grammatical gender in in bilingual Norwegian–Russian acquisition: The role of input and transparency. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ronjat, J.
(1913) Le développement du langage observé chez un enfant bilingue. Paris: Champion.Google Scholar
Serratrice, L.
(2013) Cross-linguistic influence in bilingual development: Determinants and mechanisms. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 3, 3–25. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Singleton, D.
(2001) Age and second language acquisition. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 21, 77–89. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Smithson, L., & Nicoladis, E.
(2013) Verbal memory resources predict iconic gesture use among bilinguals and monolinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16, 934–944. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Smithson, L., Paradis, J., & Nicoladis, E.
(2014) Bilingualism and receptive vocabulary achievement: Could sociocultural context make a difference? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17, 810–821. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tabouret-Keller, A.
(1962) L’acquisition du langage parlé chez un petit enfant en milieu bilingue. Problèmes de Psycho-linguistiques, 8, 205–219.Google Scholar
Thomas, E. M., & Gathercole, V. C. M.
(2007) Children’s productive command of grammatical gender and mutation in Welsh: An alternative to rule-based learning. First Language, 27, 251–278. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Thordardottir, E.
(2011) The relationship between bilingual exposure and vocabulary development. The International Journal of Bilingualism, 15, 426–445. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M.
(2009) Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Uchikoshi, Y.
(2006) English vocabulary development in bilingual kindergarteners: What are the best predictors? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 9, 33–49. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Umbel, V. M., Pearson, B. Z., Fernández, M. C., & Oller, D. K.
(1992) Measuring bilingual children’s receptive vocabularies. Child Development, 63, 1012–1020. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Unsworth, S.
(2016) Quantity and quality of language input in bilingual language development. In E. Nicoladis & S. Montarnari (Eds.), Bilingualism across the lifespan (pp. 103–121). Washington, DC: De Gruyter and American Psychological Association. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2008) Age and input in the acquisition of grammatical gender in Dutch. Second Language Research, 24, 365–395. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Vihman, M. M.
(2002) Getting started without a system: From phonetics to phonology in bilingual development. International Journal of Bilingualism, 6, 239–254. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Volterra, V., & Taeschner, T.
(1978) The acquisition and development of language by bilingual children. Journal of Child Language, 5, 311–326. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Walley, A. C.
(1993) The role of vocabulary development in children’s spoken word recognition and segmentation ability. Developmental Review, 13, 286–350. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
White, L., & Genesee, F.
(1996) How native is near-native? The issue of ultimate attainment in adult second language acquisition. Second Language Research, 12, 233–265. http://​www​.jstor​.org​/stable​/43104516
Yip, V., & Matthews, S.
(2007) The bilingual child: Early development and language contact. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 4 other publications

Dávila-Montes, José & Lyon Rathbun
2020. La Pera stories: cognitive and cultural alignment of narrative production by bilinguals in the U.S. – Mexican borderlands. Language and Intercultural Communication 20:3  pp. 240 ff. Crossref logo
Kyuchukov, Hristo, Oksana S. Ushakova & Farida S. Gazizova
2020. The Tatar-Russian Bilingualism in Early Childhood. East European Journal of Psycholinguistics 7:2 Crossref logo
Puig-Mayenco, Eloi, Ian Cunnings, Fatih Bayram, David Miller, Susagna Tubau & Jason Rothman
2018. Language Dominance Affects Bilingual Performance and Processing Outcomes in Adulthood. Frontiers in Psychology 9 Crossref logo
Rothman, Jason, Jorge González Alonso & Eloi Puig-Mayenco
2019.  In Third Language Acquisition and Linguistic Transfer, Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 12 july 2021. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.