Chapter published in:
Bilingual Cognition and Language: The state of the science across its subfields
Edited by David Miller, Fatih Bayram, Jason Rothman and Ludovica Serratrice
[Studies in Bilingualism 54] 2018
► pp. 127154
References

References

Adamson, P.
(2008) The child care transition. A league table of early childhood education and care in economically advanced countries. Florence: UNICEF Innocenti Research Centre.Google Scholar
Ameel, E., Storms, G., & Malt, B.
(2007) Semantic convergence in bilinguals. In Proceedings of the 29th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 1679), Cognitive Science Society. http://​toc​.proceedings​.com​/03510webtoc​.pdf
Arnberg, L.
(1979) Language strategies in mixed nationality families. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 20, 105–112. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bellay, C.
(2016) Musical, audio-visual, poetic, and narrative input: A longitudinal case study of French-English bilingual first language acquisition. In M. Reif & J. Robinson (Eds.), Cognitive perspectives on bilingualism (pp. 149–182). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Luk, G., Peets, K., & Yang, S.
(2010) Receptive vocabulary differences in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13, 525–531. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Blom, E., & Paradis, J.
(2015) Sources of individual differences in the acquisition of tense inflection by English second language learners with and without specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 36, 953–976. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bosch, L., & Ramon-Casas, M.
(2011) Variability in vowel production by bilingual speakers: Can input properties hinder the early stabilization of contrastive categories? Journal of Phonetics, 39, 514 – 526. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bowers, E. & Vasilyeva, M.
(2011) The relation between teacher input and lexical growth of preschoolers. Applied Psycholinguistics, 32, 221–241. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bridges, K., & Hoff, E.
(2014) Older sibling influences on the language environment and language development of toddlers in bilingual homes. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35, 225–241. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Byers-Heinlein, K.
(2013) Parental language mixing: Its measurement and the relation of mixed input to young bilingual children’s vocabulary size. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16, 32–48. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Byers-Heinlein, K., Burns, T., & Werker, J.
(2010) The roots of bilingualism in newborns. Psychological Science, 21(3), 343–348. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Byers-Heinlein, K., & Fennell, C.
(2014) Perceptual narrowing in the context of increased variation: Insights from bilingual infants. Developmental Psychobiology, 65, 274 – 291. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Carroll, S.
(2017) Exposure and input in bilingual development. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20, 3–16. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cattani, A., Abbot-Smith, K., Farag, R., Krott, A., Arreckx, F., Dennis, I., & Floccia, C.
(2014) How much exposure to English is necessary for a bilingual toddler to perform like a monolingual peer in language tests? International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 49, 649–671. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cha, K., & Goldenberg, C.
(2015) The complex relationship between bilingual home language input and kindergarten children’s Spanish and English oral proficiencies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107, 935–953. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chevalier, S.
(2015) Trilingual language acquisition. Contextual factors influencing active trilingualism in early childhood. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
De Houwer, A.
(1983) Some aspects of the simultaneous acquisition of Dutch and English by a three-year-old child. Nottingham Linguistic Circular, 12, 106–129.Google Scholar
(1990) The acquisition of two languages from birth: A case study. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1997) The role of input in the acquisition of past verb forms in English and Dutch: Evidence from a bilingual child. In E. Clark (Ed.), Proceedings of the 28th Stanford Child Language Research Forum (pp. 153–162). Stanford, CA: CSLI.Google Scholar
(1999) Environmental factors in early bilingual development: The role of parental beliefs and attitudes. In G. Extra, & L. Verhoeven (Eds.), Bilingualism and migration (pp. 75–96). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2000) Children’s linguistic environments: A first impression. In M. Beers, B. van den Bogaerde, G. Bol, J. de Jong, & C. Rooijmans (Eds.), From sound to sentence. Studies on first language acquisition (pp. 57–68). Groningen: Centre for Language and Cognition.Google Scholar
(2007) Parental language input patterns and children’s bilingual use. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 411–424. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2009) Bilingual first language acquisition. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
(2010) Assessing lexical development in Bilingual First Language Acquisition: What can we learn from monolingual norms? In M. Cruz-Ferreira (Ed.), Multilingual norms (pp. 279–322). Frankfurt: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
(2011) Language input environments and language development in bilingual acquisition. Applied Linguistics Review, 2, 221–240. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2014) The absolute frequency of maternal input to bilingual and monolingual children: A first comparison. In T. Grüter & J. Paradis (Eds.), Input and experience in bilingual development (pp. 37–58). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
(2017a) Bilingual language input environments, intake, maturity and practice. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20, 19–20. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2017b) Input, context and early child bilingualism: Implications for clinical practice. In A. Bar-On & D. Ravid (Eds.), Handbook of communication disorders: Theoretical, empirical, and applied linguistic perspectives. (pp. 599–616), Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
(2017c) Minority language parenting in Europe and children’s well-being. In N. Cabrera & B. Leyendecker (Eds.), Handbook on positive development of minority children and youth (pp. 231–246), Berlin: Springer. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
De Houwer, A., & Bornstein, M. H.
(2003, April). Balancing on the tightrope: Language use patterns in bilingual families with young children. Presentation at the International Symposium on Bilingualism, Tempe, AZ, USA.
(2016) Bilingual mothers’ language choice in child-directed speech: continuity and change. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 37, 680–693. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dixon, L. Q.
(2011) The role of home and school factors in predicting English vocabulary among bilingual kindergarten children in Singapore. Applied Psycholinguistics, 32, 141–168. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Döpke, S.
(1986) Discourse structures in bilingual families. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 7, 493–507. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1992) One parent one language: An interactional approach. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Garlin, E.
(2008) Bilingualer Erstspracherwerb: Sprachlich handeln, Sprachprobieren, Sprachreflexion. Eine Langzeitstudie eines deutsch-spanisch aufwachsenden Geschwisterpaares. Münster: Waxmann.Google Scholar
Gathercole, V. C. M.
(2014) Bilingualism matters: One size does not fit all. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 38, 359 – 366. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, V. M., & Hoff, E.
(2007) Input and the acquisition of language: Three questions. In E. Hoff & M. Shatz (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of language development (pp. 107–127). Oxford: Blackwell. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Genesee, F., Boivin, I., & Nicoladis, E.
(1996) Talking with strangers: A study of bilingual children’s communicative competence. Applied Psycholinguistics, 17, 427 – 442. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Genesee, F., & Delcenserie, A.
(Eds.) (2016) Starting over – The language development in internationally-adopted children. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gervain, J., & Werker, J.
(2013) Prosody cues word order in 7-month-old bilingual infants. Nature Communications, 4,1490. Crossref.Google Scholar
Gleitman, L., & Newport, E.
(1995) The invention of language by children: Environmental and biological influences on the acquisition of language. In L. R. Gleitman & M. Liberman (Eds.), An invitation to cognitive science, Vol. 1: Language (2nd ed., pp. 1–24). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Goodz, N.
(1989) Parental language mixing in bilingual families. Infant Mental Health Journal, 10, 25 – 44. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Grosjean, F.
(2015) One Person–One Language and bilingual children. The amazing beginning of modern OPOL. Available at https://​www​.psychologytoday​.com​/blog​/life​-bilingual​/201504​/one​-person​-one​-language​-and​-bilingual​-children (17 March, 2017).
Grüter, T., & Paradis, J.
Eds. Input and experience in bilingual development Amsterdam John Benjamins Crossref
Hammer, C. S., Komaroff, E., Rodriguez, B., Lopez, L., Scarpino, S., & Goldstein, B.
(2012) Predicting Spanish-English bilingual children’s language abilities. Journal of Speech-Language-Hearing Research, 55, 1251–1264. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hart, B., & Risley, T.
(1995) Meaningful differences in the everyday experiences of young American children. Baltimore, MD: Paul Brookes.Google Scholar
Hoff, E., Core, C., Place, S., Rumiche, R., Señor, M., & Parra, M.
(2012) Dual language exposure and early bilingual development. Journal of Child Language, 39, 1–27. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hoff, E., Rumiche, R., Burridge, A., Ribot, K., & Welsh, S.
(2014) Expressive vocabulary development in children from bilingual and monolingual homes: A longitudinal study from two to four years. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29, 433–444. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hoff, E., & Tian, C.
(2005) Socioeconomic status and cultural influences on language. Journal of Communication Disorders, 38, 271–278. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hurtado, N., Grüter, T., Marchman, V., & Fernald, A.
(2014) Relative language exposure, processing efficiency and vocabulary in Spanish–English bilingual toddlers. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17, 189–202. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hyams, N.
(1991) Seven not-so-trivial trivia of language acquisition: Comments on the paper by Wolfgang Klein. In L. Eubank (Ed.), Point counterpoint, Universal Grammar in the second language (pp. 71–88). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Juan-Garau, M., & Pérez-Vidal, C.
(2001) Mixing and pragmatic parental strategies in early bilingual acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 28, 59–86. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kasuya, H.
(1998) Determinants of language choice in bilingual children: The role of input. International Journal of Bilingualism, 2, 327 – 346. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kibler, A., Palacios, N., Simpson-Baird, A.
(2014) The influence of older siblings on language use among second-generation Latino preschoolers. TESOL Quarterly, 48, 164–175. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kibler, A., Palacios, N., Simpson-Baird, A., Bergey, R., & Yoder, M.
(2016) Bilingual Latin at children’s exposure to language and literacy practices through older siblings in immigrant families. Linguistics and Education, 35, 66–77. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Klein, W.
(1991) Seven trivia of language acquisition. In L. Eubank (Ed.), Point counterpoint: Universal Grammar in the second language (pp. 49–70). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lanza, E.
(1988) Language strategies in the home: linguistic input and infant bilingualism. In A. Holmen, E. Hansen, J. Gimbel, & N. Jørgensen (Eds.), Bilingualism and the individual (pp. 69–84). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
(1992) Can bilingual two-year-olds code-switch? Journal of Child Language, 19, 633 – 658. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1997) Language mixing in infant bilingualism. A sociolinguistic perspective. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Li Wei
(2011) The early acquisition of English as a second language: The case of young Chinese learners of English in Britain. In A. De Houwer & A. Wilton (Eds.), English in Europe today: Educational and sociocultural perspectives (pp. 105–122). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Long, D., & Rothman, J.
(2014) Some caveats to the role of input in the timing of child bilingualism. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 4, 351–356. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Marchman, V., Martínez, L., Hurtado, N., Grüter, T., & Fernald, A.
(2016) Caregiver talk to young Spanish-English bilinguals: Comparing direct observation and parent-report measures of dual-language exposure. Developmental Science. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Meisel, J.
(1989) Early differentiation of languages in bilingual children. In K. Hyltenstam & L. Obler (Eds.), Bilingualism across the lifespan: Aspects of acquisition, maturity and loss (pp. 13–40), Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mishra, S., & Mishra, A.
(2016) Linguistic interference from Hindi in Indian English. International Journal on Studies in English Language and Literature, 4, 29–38.Google Scholar
Montrul, S.
(2008) Incomplete acquisition in bilingualism. Re-examining the age factor. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nakamura, J.
(2016) Hidden bilingualism: Ideological influences on the language practices of multilingual migrant mothers in Japan. International Multilingual Research Journal, 10, 308–323. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Newman, R., Rowe, M., & Bernstein Ratner, N.
(2016) Input and uptake at 7 months predicts toddler vocabulary: the role of child-directed speech and infant processing skills in language development. Journal of Child Language, 43, 967–968. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Oades-Sese, G., & Yibling, L.
(2011) Attachment relationships as predictors of language skills for at-risk bilingual preschool children. Psychology in the Schools. Special Issue: Resilience in the School, 48, 707–722. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Palacios, N., Kibler, A., Simpson Baird, A., Parr, A., & Bergey, R.
(2015) An examination of language practices during mother-child play activities among Mexican immigrant families. International Multilingual Research Journal, 9, 197–219. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J.
(2017) Parent report data on input and experience reliably predict bilingual development and this is not trivial. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20, 27–28. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J. & Genesee, F.
(1996) Syntactic acquisition in bilingual children: autonomous or interdependent? Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 18, 1–25. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J., & Kirova, A.
(2014) English second-language learners in preschool: Profile effects in their English abilities and the role of home language environment. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 38, 342–349. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J., & Navarro, S.
(2003) Subject realization and crosslinguistic interference in the bilingual acquisition of Spanish and English: what is the role of the input? Journal of Child Language, 30, 371 – 393. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J., Nicoladis, E., Crago, M., & Genesee, F.
(2011) Bilingual children’s acquisition of the past tense: A usage-based approach. Journal of Child Language, 38, 554 – 578. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J., Tremblay, A., & Crago, M.
(2008) Bilingual children’s acquisition of English inflection: The role of language dominance and task type. In H. Chan, H. Jacob, & E. Kapia (Eds.), BUCLD 32: Proceedings of the 32nd annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 378–389). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Parra, M., Hoff, E., & Core, C.
(2011) Relations among language exposure, phonological memory, and language development in Spanish-English bilingually developing two year olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 113–125. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pearson, B. Z.
(2007) Social factors in childhood bilingualism in the United States. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 399–410. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pearson, B. Z., Fernández, S., Lewedeg, V., & Oller, D. K.
(1997) The relation of input factors to lexical learning by bilingual infants. Applied Psycholinguistics, 18, 41–58. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pierce, L., Genesee, F., Gauthier, K., & Dubois, M. E.
(2015) Communication patterns between parents and children: Comparing mothers and fathers in different learner contexts. Applied Psycholinguistics, 36, 1223–1246. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pirvulescu, M., Pérez-Leroux, A., & Roberge, Y.
(2012) A bidirectional study of object omissions in French-English bilinguals. In K. Braunmüller & C. Gabriel (Eds.), Multilingual individuals and multilingual societies (pp. 171–188). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Place, S., & Hoff, E.
(2011) Properties of dual language exposure that influence two-year-olds’ bilingual proficiency. Child Development, 82, 1834–1849. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2016) Effects and noneffects of input in bilingual environments on dual language skills in 2 1⁄2- year-olds. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19, 1023–1041. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ribot, K., & Hoff, E.
(2014) “¿Cómo estas?” “I’m good.” Conversational code-switching is related to profiles of expressive and receptive proficiency in Spanish-English bilingual toddlers. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 38, 333 – 341. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rieckborn, S.
(2006) The development of forms and functions in the acquisition of tense and aspect in German–French bilingual children. In C. Lleó (Ed.), Interfaces in multilingualism (pp. 61–89). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rodina, Y., & Westergaard, M.
(2017) Grammatical gender in bilingual Norwegian–Russian acquisition: The role of input and transparency. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20, 197–214. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ronjat, J.
(1913) Le développement du langage observé chez un enfant bilingue. Paris: Champion.Google Scholar
Scheele, A., Leseman, P., & Mayo, A.
(2010) The home language environment of monolingual and bilingual children and their language proficiency. Applied Psycholinguistics, 31, 117–140. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schulz, P. & Tracy, R.
(2011) LiSe-DaZ. Linguistische Sprachstandserhebung – Deutsch als Zweitsprache. (In Verbindung mit der Baden-Württemberg Stiftung). Göttingen: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Schwartz, M. & Rovner, H.
(2015) The acquisition of definiteness in Hebrew (L2) by bilingual preschool children (Russian-L1): A longitudinal multiple-case study. International Journal of Bilingualism, 19, 548–571. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Serratrice, L.
To appear). Early childhood. In A. De Houwer & L. Ortega Eds. The Cambridge handbook of bilingualism Cambridge Cambridge University Press
Silva-Corvalán, C.
(2014) Bilingual language acquisition. Spanish and English in the first six years. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Silva-Corvalán, C. & Montanari, S.
(2008) The acquisition of ser, estar (and be) by a Spanish–English bilingual child: The early stages. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11, 341 – 360. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Silvén, M., Voeten, M., Kouvo, A., & Lundén, M.
(2014) Speech perception and vocabulary growth: A longitudinal study of Finnish-Russian bilinguals and Finnish monolinguals from infancy to three years. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 38, 323 – 332. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Slavkov, N.
(2015) Language attrition and reactivation in the context of bilingual first language acquisition. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 18, 715–734. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Song, L., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Yoshikawa, H., Kahana-Kalman, R., & Wu, I.
(2012) Language experiences and vocabulary development in Dominican and Mexican infants across the first 2 years. Developmental Psychology, 48, 1106–1123. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sorace, A.
(2005) Selective optionality in language development. In L. Cornips & N. Corrigan (Eds.), Syntax and variation. Reconciling the biological and the social (pp. 55–80). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tare, M., & Gelman, S.
(2011) Bilingual parents’ modeling of pragmatic language use in multiparty interactions. Applied Psycholinguistics, 32, 761–780. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M.
(2016) Jerome Seymour Bruner [1915–2016]. Journal of Child Language, 43, 967–968. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Unsworth, S.
(2013) Assessing the role of current and cumulative exposure in simultaneous bilingual acquisition: The case of Dutch gender. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16, 86–110. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2016) Quantity and quality of language input in bilingual language development. In E. Nicoladis & S. Montanari (Eds.), Lifespan perspectives on bilingualism (pp. 136–196). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter/APA. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
van de Weijer, J.
(2000) Language input and word discovery. In M. Beers, B. van den Bogaerde, G. Bol, J. de Jong, & C. Rooijmans (Eds.), From sound to sentence: Studies on first language acquisition (pp. 155–162). Groningen: Centre for Language and Cognition.Google Scholar
Weisleder, A., & Fernald, A.
(2013) Talking to children matters: early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychological Science, 24, 2143–2152. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2014) Social environments shape children’s language experiences, strengthening language processing and building vocabulary. In I. Arnon, M. Casillas, C. Kurumada, & B. Estigarribia (Eds.), Language in interaction. Studies in honor of Eve V. Clark (pp. 29–49). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Wells, G.
(1986) Variation in child language. In P. Fletcher & M. Garman (Eds.), Language acquisition: Studies in first language development (pp. 109–139). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wijnen, F.
(2000) Input, intake and sequence in syntactic development. In M. Beers, B. van de Bogaerde, G. Bol, J. de Jong, & C. Rooijmans (Eds.), From sound to sentence – Studies on first language acquisition (pp. 163–186). Groningen: Centre for Language and Cognition.Google Scholar
Winsler, A., Burchinal, M., Tien, H.-C., Peisner-Feinberg, E., Espinosa, L., Castro, D., LaForett, D., Kim, Y., & De Feyter, J.
(2014) Early developmental skills of diverse dual language learners: The roles of home language use, cultural heritage, maternal immigration, and sociodemographics. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29, 750–764. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Yamamoto, M.
(2001) Language use in interlingual families: A Japanese–English sociolinguistic study. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
(2005) What makes who choose what language to whom?: Language use in Japanese-Filipino interlingual families in Japan. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 8, 588 – 606. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 15 other publications

Carbajal, Maria Julia & Sharon Peperkamp
2020. Dual language input and the impact of language separation on early lexical development. Infancy 25:1  pp. 22 ff. Crossref logo
De Houwer, Annick
2019.  In The Encyclopedia of Applied Linguistics,  pp. 1 ff. Crossref logo
Annick De Houwer & Lourdes Ortega
2018.  In The Cambridge Handbook of Bilingualism, Crossref logo
Dubiel, Bozena & Eithne Guilfoyle
2021. Early shifts in the heritage language strength: A comparison of lexical access and accuracy in bilingual and monolingual children. International Journal of Bilingualism 25:1  pp. 21 ff. Crossref logo
Ehl, Birgit & Michael Grosche
2020. Einbezug von Alter und Sprachkontaktdauer in die Wortschatzdiagnostik bei Mehrsprachigkeit. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie 52:1-2  pp. 41 ff. Crossref logo
Kyuchukov, Hristo, Oksana S. Ushakova & Farida S. Gazizova
2020. The Tatar-Russian Bilingualism in Early Childhood. East European Journal of Psycholinguistics 7:2 Crossref logo
Lloyd-Smith, Anika, Fatih Bayram & Michael Iverson
2020.  In Studies in Turkish as a Heritage Language [Studies in Bilingualism, 60],  pp. 63 ff. Crossref logo
Ortega, Lourdes
2020. The Study of Heritage Language Development From a Bilingualism and Social Justice Perspective. Language Learning 70:S1  pp. 15 ff. Crossref logo
Perpiñán, Silvia & Adriana Soto-Corominas
2021. Indirect structural crosslinguistic influence in early Catalan–Spanish bilinguals in adulthood: Predicate selection in Catalan existential constructions. Applied Psycholinguistics  pp. 1 ff. Crossref logo
Serrat, Elisabet, Anna Amadó, Aina Bonet, Sara Feijóo & Eva Aguilar-Mediavilla
2021. Growing up in a socially bilingual environment: simultaneous acquisition of a typologically close language (Crecer en un entorno social bilingüe: adquisición de una lengua de forma simultánea a otra lengua tipológicamente similar). Journal for the Study of Education and Development 44:2  pp. 336 ff. Crossref logo
Smolander, Sini, Marja Laasonen, Eva Arkkila, Pekka Lahti‐Nuuttila & Sari Kunnari
2021. L2 vocabulary acquisition of early sequentially bilingual children with TD and DLD affected differently by exposure and age of onset. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders 56:1  pp. 72 ff. Crossref logo
SUN, He, Siew Chin NG, Beth Ann O'BRIEN & Tom FRITZSCHE
2020. Child, family, and school factors in bilingual preschoolers’ vocabulary development in heritage languages. Journal of Child Language 47:4  pp. 817 ff. Crossref logo
Vaahtoranta, Enni, Sebastian Suggate, Jan Lenhart & Wolfgang Lenhard
2021. Language exposure and phonological short-term memory as predictors of majority language vocabulary and phonological awareness in dual language learning. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 24:2  pp. 319 ff. Crossref logo
Wang, Weihong & Xiao Lan Curdt-Christiansen
2021. Lost in translation: parents as medium translators in intergenerational language transmission. Current Issues in Language Planning 22:4  pp. 362 ff. Crossref logo
Wu, Ruilin & Esli Struys
2021. Language Dominance and Sociolinguistic Experience Are Related to Language Control and Domain-General Monitoring Control: An Investigation in Bilinguals Who Live in a Minority/Majority Sociolinguistic Setting. Frontiers in Psychology 12 Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 14 september 2021. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.