Chapter published in:
Meaning and Structure in Second Language Acquisition: In honor of Roumyana Slabakova
Edited by Jacee Cho, Michael Iverson, Tiffany Judy, Tania Leal and Elena Shimanskaya
[Studies in Bilingualism 55] 2018
► pp. 123148
References

References

Anderson, B.
(2007) Learnability and parametric change in the nominal system of L2 French. Language Acquisition, 14(2), 165–214.CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2008) Forms of evidence and grammatical development in the acquisition of adjective position in L2 French. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 30, 1–30.CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Androutsopoulou, A., Español-Echevarría, M., & Prévost, P.
(2008) On the acquisition of the prenominal placement of evaluative adjectives in L2 Spanish. In J. Bruhn de Garavito & E. Valenzuela (Eds.), Proceedings of the 10th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium (pp. 1–12). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Baker, M.
(2008) The macroparameter in a microparametric world. In T. Biberauer (Ed.), The limits of syntactic variation (pp. 351–374). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bernstein, J.
(1991) Nominal enclitics in Romance. MIT Working Papers in Linguistics, 14, 51–66.Google Scholar
(1992) On the syntactic status of adjectives in Romance. CUNYForum, 17, 105–122.Google Scholar
(2001) Focusing the “right” way in Romance determiner phrases. Probus, 13, 1–30.CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bley-Vroman, R.
(1983) The comparative fallacy in interlanguage studies: The case of systematicity. Language Learning, 33, 1–17.CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Borer, H.
(1984) Parametric syntax: Case studies in Semitic and Romance languages. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar
Carstens, V.
(1991) The morphology and syntax of determiner phrase in Kiswahili (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of California, LA.Google Scholar
Cinque, G.
(1994) On the evidence for partial N-movement in the Romance DP. In G. Cinque, J. Koster, J-Y. Pollock, L. Rizzi, & R. Zanuttini (Eds.), Paths towards Universal Grammar: Studies in honor of Richard S. Kayne (pp. 85–110). Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
(2010) The syntax of adjectives: A comparative study. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N.
(1995) The minimalist program. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H., Penke, M., & Parodi, T.
(1993/1994) Functional categories in early child German. Language Acquisition, 3, 395–429.CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Crisma, P.
(1991) Functional categories inside the noun phrase: A study of the distribution nominal modifiers (Unpublished tesi di laurea). Università di Venezia.Google Scholar
(1996) On the configurational nature of adjectival modification. In K. Zagona (Ed.), Grammatical theory and Romance languages (pp. 59–71). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Eisenbeiss, S.
(2003) Feature-driven structure building. A study on the acquisition of noun-phrase and inflection (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Düsseldorf.Google Scholar
Hawkins, R., & Chan, C.
(1997) The partial accessibility of Universal Grammar in second language acquisition: The failed functional features hypothesis. Second Language Research, 13, 187–226.CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hawkins, R., & Hattori, H.
(2006) Interpretation of English multiple wh-questions by Japanese speakers, a missing uninterpretable feature account. Second Language Research, 22, 269–301.CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Judy, T., Guijarro-Fuentes, P., & Rothman, J.
(2008) Adult accessibility to L2 representational features: Evidence from the Spanish DP. In M. Bowles, R. Foote, S. Perpiñán, & R. Bhatt (Eds.), Selected proceedings of Second Language Research Forum 2007 (pp. 1–21). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Lardiere, D.
(1998a) Case and tense in the ‘fossilized’ steady state. Second Language Research, 14, 1–26.CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1998b) Dissociating syntax from morphology in a divergent end-state grammar. Second Language Research, 14, 359–375.CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2005) On morphological competence. In L. Dekydtspotter, R. Sprouse, & A. Liljestrand (Eds.), Proceedings of the 7th Generative Approaches to Second Language Acquisition Conference (GASLA 2004) (pp. 178–192). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
(2009) Some thoughts on the contrastive analysis of features in second language acquisition. Second Language Research, 25, 173–227.CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Manzini, R., & Wexler, K.
(1987) Parameters, binding theory, and learnability. Linguistic Inquiry, 18, 413–444.Google Scholar
Parodi, T., Schwartz, B., & Clashen, H.
(2004) On the L2 acquisition of the morphosyntax of German nominals. Linguistics, 42(3), 669–705.CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Penner, Z.
(1993) The earliest stage in the acquisition of the nominal phrase in Bernese Swiss German: syntactic bootstrapping and the architecture of language learning (Arbeit-spapier 30). Bern: University of Bern.Google Scholar
Penner, Z., & Schönenberger, M.
(1992) The distribution of nominal agreement features in Swiss-German dialects and the strong DP/CP parallelism hypothesis. In Z. Penner (Ed.), Topics in Swiss grammar syntax (pp. 331–346). Bern: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Picallo, M. C.
(1991) Nominals and nominaliziation in Catalan. Probus, 3, 279–316.CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pienemann, M.
(1998) Language processing and second language development: Processability theory. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Radford, A.
(1990) Syntactic theory and the acquisition of English syntax. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Reinhart, T.
(2006) Interface strategies. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Ritter, E.
(1991) Two functional categories in noun phrases: Evidence from Modern Hebrew. Syntax and Semantics, 26, 37–62.Google Scholar
Roehrs, D.
(2006) The morpho-syntax of the Germanic noun phrase: Determiners Move into the determiner phrase (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Indiana University, Bloomington, IN.Google Scholar
Rothman, J., Iverson, M., Judy, T., & Guijarro-Fuentes, P.
(2009) Noun-raising and adjectival interpretative reflexes in the L2 Spanish of Germanic and Italian learners. In J. Chandlee, M. Franchini, S. Lord, & G. M. Rheiner (Eds.), BUCLD 33: Proceedings of the 33rd annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 444–455). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Schwartz, B., & Sprouse, R.
(1996) L2 cognitive states and the full transfer/full access model. Second Language Research, 12, 40–72.CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Slabakova, R.
(2012) Acquisition of temporal meanings without temporal morphology: The case of Mandarin Chinese. Paper presented at the Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition North America (GALANA) conference, University of Kansas, Lawrence, KA.
(2013) What is easy and what is hard in second language acquisition: A generative perspective. In M. P. García Mayo, M. J. Gutierrez-Mangado, & M. Martínez Adrián (Eds.), Contemporary approaches to second language acquisition (pp. 5–28). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2014) The bottleneck of second language acquisition. Foreign Language Teaching and Research, 46(4), 543–559.Google Scholar
(2016) Second Language Acquisition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sorace, A.
(2011) Pinning down the concept of “interface” in bilingualism. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 1(1), 1–33.CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Valois, D.
(1991) The internal syntax of DP (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of California, LA.Google Scholar
White, L.
(2003) Second language acquisition and universal grammar. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Zagona, K.
(2002) The syntax of Spanish. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar