Part of
Bilingualism, Executive Function, and Beyond: Questions and insights
Edited by Irina A. Sekerina, Lauren Spradlin and Virginia Valian
[Studies in Bilingualism 57] 2019
► pp. 1734
References
Abutalebi, J., Della Rosa, P. A., Green, D. W., Hernandez, M., Scifo, P., Keim, R., … Costa, A.
(2012) Bilingualism tunes the anterior cingulate cortex for conflict monitoring. Cerebral Cortex, 22, 2076–2086. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Adi-Japha, E., Berberich-Artzi, J., & Libnawi, A.
(2010) Cognitive flexibility in drawings of bilingual children. Child Development, 81, 1356–1366. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Altman, D. G., & Bland, J. M.
(1995) Absence of evidence is not evidence of absence. British Medical Journal, 311, 485. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Green, D. W., & Gollan, T. H.
(2009) Bilingual minds. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 10, 89–129. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M.
(2004) Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19, 290–303. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Luk, G., Peets, K. F., & Yang, S.
(2010) Receptive vocabulary differences in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13, 525–531. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Martin, M. M., & Viswanathan, M.
(2005) Bilingualism across the lifespan: The rise and fall of inhibitory control. International Journal of Bilingualism, 9, 103–119. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Blom, E., Kuntay, A. C., Messer, M., Verhagen, J., & Leseman, P.
(2014) The benefits of being bilingual: Working memory in bilingual Turkish-Dutch children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 128C, 105–119. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Blumenfeld, H. K., & Marian, V.
(2014) Cognitive control in bilinguals: Advantages in Stimulus-Stimulus inhibition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17, 610–629. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Calvo, A., & Bialystok, E.
(2014) Independent effects of bilingualism and socioeconomic status on language ability and executive functioning. Cognition, 130, 278–288. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carlson, S. M., & Meltzoff, A. N.
(2008) Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental Science, 11, 282–298. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Colzato, L. S., Bajo, M. T., van den Wildenberg, W., Paolieri, D., Nieuwenhuis, S., La Heij, W., & Hommel, B.
(2008) How does bilingualism improve executive control? A comparison of active and reactive inhibition mechanisms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 34, 302–312.Google Scholar
Costa, A., Hernández, M., & Sebastian-Galles, N.
(2008) Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: evidence from the ANT task. Cognition, 106, 59–86. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
de Bruin, A., Treccani, B., & Della Sala, S.
(2015) Cognitive advantage in bilingualism: an example of publication bias? Psychological Science, 26, 99–107. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dunabeitia, J. A., Hernandez, J. A., Anton, E., Macizo, P., Estevez, A., Fuentes, L. J., & Carreiras, M.
(2014) The inhibitory advantage in bilingual children revisited: myth or reality? Experimental Psychology, 61, 234–251. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Engel de Abreu, P. M., Cruz-Santos, A., Tourinho, C. J., Martin, R., & Bialystok, E.
(2012) Bilingualism enriches the poor: Enhanced cognitive control in low-income minority children. Psychological Science, 23, 1364–1371. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Engle, R. W., Laughlin, J. E., Tuholski, S. W., & Conway, A. R.
(1999) Working Memory, Short-Term Memory, and General Fluid Intellignece: A Latent-Variable Approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128, 309–331. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Esposito, A. G., Baker-Ward, L., & Mueller, S. T.
(2013) Interference suppression vs. response inhibition: An explanation for the absence of a bilingual advantage in preschoolers’ Stroop task performance. Cognitive Development, 28, 354–363. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Filippi, R., Morris, J. C., Richardson, F. M., Bright, P., Thomas, M. S. C., Karmiloff-Smith, A., & Marian, V.
(2015) Bilingual children show an advantage in controlling verbal interference during spoken language comprehension. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18, 490–501. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fisher, R. A.
(1935) The design of experiments. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Gathercole, V. C., Thomas, E. M., Kennedy, I., Prys, C., Young, N., Viñas Guasch, N., … Jones, L.
(2014) Does language dominance affect cognitive performance in bilinguals? Lifespan evidence from preschoolers through older adults on card sorting, Simon, and metalinguistic tasks. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 11. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gold, B. T., Johnson, N. F., & Powell, D. K.
(2013) Lifelong bilingualism contributes to cognitive reserve against white matter integrity declines in aging. Neuropsychologia, 51, 2841–2846. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grady, C. L., Luk, G., Craik, F. I., & Bialystok, E.
(2015) Brain network activity in monolingual and bilingual older adults. Neuropsychologia, 66, 170–181. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hervais-Adelman, A. G., Moser-Mercer, B., & Golestani, N.
(2011) Executive control of language in the bilingual brain: integrating the evidence from neuroimaging to neuropsychology. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 234. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hilchey, M. D., & Klein, R. M.
(2011) Are there bilingual advantages on nonlinguistic interference tasks? Implications for the plasticity of executive control processes. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 18, 625–658. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kalia, V., Wilbourn, M. P., & Ghio, K.
(2014) Better early or late? Examining the influence of age of exposure and language proficiency on executive function in early and late bilinguals. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 26, 699–713. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kempert, S., Saalbach, H., & Hardy, I.
(2011) Cognitive benefits and costs of bilingualism in elementary school students: The case of mathematical word problems. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103, 547–561. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kousaie, S., & Phillips, N. A.
(2012) Conflict monitoring and resolution: are two languages better than one? Evidence from reaction time and event-related brain potentials. Brain Research, 1446, 71–90. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kousaie, S., Sheppard, C., Lemieux, M., Monetta, L., & Taler, V.
(2014) Executive function and bilingualism in young and older adults. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 250. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kramer, A. F., Bherer, L., Colcombe, S. J., Dong, W., & Greenough, W. T.
(2004) Environmental influences on cognitive and brain plasticity during aging. Journal of Gerontology:Medical Sciences, 59A, 940–957.Google Scholar
Kramer, A. F., & Erickson, K. I.
(2007) Capitalizing on cortical plasticity: influence of physical activity on cognition and brain function. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 342–348. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Krizman, J., Skoe, E., & Kraus, N.
(2016) Bilingual enhancements have no socioeconomic boundaries. Developmental Science, 19, 881–891. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J. F., & Bialystok, E.
(2013) Understanding the consequences of bilingualism for language processing and cognition. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 497–514. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J. F., Dussias, P. E., Bogulski, C. A., & Valdes-Kroff, J.
(2012) Juggling two languages in one mind: What bilinguals tell us about language processing and its consequences for cognition. In B. Ross (Ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation (Vol. 56, pp. 229–262). San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Li, L., Abutalebi, J., Zou, L., Yan, X., Liu, L., Feng, X., … Ding, G.
(2015) Bilingualism alters brain functional connectivity between “control” regions and “language” regions: Evidence from bimodal bilinguals. Neuropsychologia, 71, 236–247. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Luk, G.
(2008) The anatomy of the bilingual influence on cognition: Levels of functional use and proficiency of language. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, York University, Toronto, Canada.Google Scholar
Luk, G., Anderson, J. A. E., Craik, F. I. M., Grady, C., & Bialystok, E.
(2010) Distinct neural correlates for two types of inhibition in bilinguals: Response inhibition and intereference supression. Brain and Cognition, 74, 347–357. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Luk, G., & Bialystok, E.
(2013) Bilingualism is not a categorical variable: Interaction between language proficiency and usage. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 605–621. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Luk, G., Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Grady, C. L.
(2011) Lifelong bilingualism maintains white matter integrity in older adults. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 16808–16813. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marzecova, A., Asanowicz, D., Kriva, L., & Wodniecka, Z.
(2013) The effects of bilingualism on efficiency and lateralization of attentional networks. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16, 608–623. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mechelli, A., Crinion, J. T., Noppeney, U., O’Doherty, J., Ashburner, J., Frackowiak, R. S., & Price, C. J.
(2004) Structural plasticity in the bilingual brain. Nature, 431, 757. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mezzacappa, E.
(2004) Alerting, orienting, and executive attention: Developmental properties and sociodemographic correlates in an epidemiological sample of young, urban children. Child Development, 75, 1373–1386. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D.
(2000) The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morales, J., Yudes, C., Gomez-Ariza, C. J., & Bajo, M. T.
(2014) Bilingualism modulates dual mechanisms of cognitive control: Evidence from ERPs. Neuropsychologia, 66C, 157–169.Google Scholar
Morton, J. P., & Harper, S. N.
(2007) What did Simon say? Revisiting the bilingual advantage. Developmental Science, 10, 719–726. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Noble, K. G., Norman, M. F., & Farah, M. J.
(2005) Neurocognitive correlates of socioeconomic status in kindergarten children. Developmental Science, 8, 74–87. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paap, K. R., & Greenberg, Z. I.
(2013) There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology, 66, 232–258. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paap, K. R., Johnson, H. A., & Sawi, O.
(2015a) Are bilingual advantages dependent upon specific tasks or specific bilingual experiences? Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 26, 615–639. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015b) Bilingual advantages in executive functioning either do not exist or are restricted to very specific and undetermined circumstances. Cortex, 69, 265–78. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pelham, S. D., & Abrams, L.
(2014) Cognitive Advantages and Disadvantages in Early and Late Bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40, 313–325.Google Scholar
Pliatsikas, C., Moschopoulou, E., & Saddy, J. D.
(2015) The effects of bilingualism on the white matter structure of the brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112, 1334–1337. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Prior, A., & Macwhinney, B.
(2010) A bilingual advantage in task switching. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13, 253–262. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Silver, N.
(2012) The signal and the noise: Why so many predictions fail – but some don’t. New York: Penguin.Google Scholar
Tao, L., Marzecova, A., Taft, M., Asanowicz, D., & Wodniecka, Z.
(2011) The efficiency of attentional networks in early and late bilinguals: the role of age of acquisition. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 123. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Treccani, B., Argyri, E., Sorace, A., & Sala, S. D.
(2009) Spatial negative priming in bilingualism. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 16, 320–327. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
von Bastian, C. C., Souza, A. S., & Gade, M.
(2016) No evidence for bilingual cognitive advantages: A test of four hypotheses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145, 246–258. doi: DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yang, S., Yang, H., & Lust, B.
(2011) Early childhood bilingualism leads to advances in executive attention: Dissociating culture and language. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14, 412–422. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zhou, B., & Krott, A.
(2016) Data trimming procedure can eliminate bilingual cognitive advantage. Psychonomics Bulletin and Review, 23, 1221–1230. DOI logo.Google Scholar