Part of
L3 Development After the Initial State
Edited by Megan M. Brown-Bousfield, Suzanne Flynn and Éva Fernández-Berkes
[Studies in Bilingualism 65] 2023
► pp. 4971
References
Abercrombie, D.
(1967) Elements of general phonetics. Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Ahn, S., Chang, C. B., DeKeyser, R., & Lee-Ellis, S.
(2017) Age effects in first language attrition: Speech perception by Korean-English bilinguals. Language Learning, 67(3), 694–733. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Anderson-Hsieh, J., Johnson, R., & Koehler, K.
(1992) The relationship between native speaker judgments of nonnative pronunciation and deviance in segmentals, prosody, and syllable structure. Language Learning, 42(4), 529–555. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Arvaniti, A.
(2012) The usefulness of metrics in the quantification of speech rhythm. Journal of Phonetics, 40(3), 351–373. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bardel, C., & Falk, Y.
(2007) The role of the second language in third language acquisition: The case of Germanic syntax. Second Language Research, 23(4), 459–484. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012) The L2 status factor and the declarative/procedural distinction. In J. Cabrelli, S. Flynn, & J. Rothman (Eds.), Third language acquisition in adulthood (pp. 61–78). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boersma, P., & Weenink, D.
(2009) Praat: Doing phonetics by computer (Version 5.1.13). University of Amsterdam. Retrieved on 2 June 2023 from [URL]
Brown, M. M.
(2020) Grammatical gender acquisition in sequential trilinguals: Influence of a gendered L1 vs. L2. Proceedings of the Linguistic Society of America, 5(1), 331–344. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bylund, E.
(2009) Effects of age of L2 acquisition on L1 event conceptualization patterns. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12(3), 305–322. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cabrelli Amaro, J.
(2017) Testing the Phonological Permeability Hypothesis: L3 phonological effects on L1 versus L2 systems. International Journal of Bilingualism, 21(6), 698–717. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cabrelli Amaro, J., & Rothman, J.
(2010) On L3 acquisition and phonological permeability: A new test case for debates on the mental representation of non-native phonological systems. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 48(2–3), 275–296.Google Scholar
Cabrelli Amaro, J., & Wrembel, M.
(2016) Investigating the acquisition of phonology in a third language – A state of the science and an outlook for the future. International Journal of Multilingualism, 13(4), 395–409. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chan, I. L., & Chang, C. B.
(2019) Perception of nonnative tonal contrasts by Mandarin-English and English-Mandarin sequential bilinguals. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 146(2), 956–972. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chang, C. B.
(2010) First language phonetic drift during second language acquisition (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of California at Berkeley.
(2019a) Language change and linguistic inquiry in a world of multicompetence: Sustained phonetic drift and its implications for behavioral linguistic research. Journal of Phonetics, 74, 96–113. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2019b) Phonetic drift. In M. S. Schmid & B. Köpke (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of language attrition (pp. 191–203). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
De Leeuw, E.
(2019) Phonetic attrition. In M. S. Schmid & B. Köpke (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of language attrition (pp. 204–217). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
De Leeuw, E., Mennen, I., & Scobbie, J. M.
(2012) Singing a different tune in your native language: First language attrition of prosody. International Journal of Bilingualism, 16(1), 101–116. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
De Leeuw, E., Tusha, A., & Schmid, M. S.
(2018) Individual phonological attrition in Albanian-English late bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21(2), 278–295. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dellwo, V.
(2006) Rhythm and speech rate: A variation coefficient for ∆C. In P. Karnowski & I. Szigeti (Eds.), Language and language processing: Proceedings of the 38th Linguistic Colloquium (pp. 231–241). Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Falk, Y., & Bardel, C.
(2010) The study of the role of the background languages in third language acquisition: The state of the art. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 43(2), 185–219. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011) Object pronouns in German L3 syntax: Evidence for the L2 status factor. Second Language Research, 27(1), 59–82. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Flege, J. E.
(1980) Phonetic approximation in second language acquisition. Language Learning, 30(1), 117–134. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1987) The production of “new” and “similar” phones in a foreign language: Evidence for the effect of equivalence classification. Journal of Phonetics, 15(1), 47–65. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1995) Second language speech learning: Theory, findings, and problems. In W. Strange (Ed.), Speech perception and linguistic experience: Issues in cross-language research (pp. 229–273). York Press.Google Scholar
Flege, J. E., Birdsong, D. P., Bialystok, E., Mack, M., Sung, H., & Tsukada, K.
(2006) Degree of foreign accent in English sentences produced by Korean children and adults. Journal of Phonetics, 34(2), 153–175. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Flege, J. E., & Eefting, W.
(1987) Cross-language switching in stop consonant perception and production by Dutch speakers of English. Speech Communication, 6(3), 185–202. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Giancaspro, D., Halloran, B., & Iverson, M.
(2015) Transfer at the initial stages of L3 Brazilian Portuguese: A look at three groups of English/Spanish bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(2), 191–207. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grabe, E., & Low, E. L.
(2002) Durational variability in speech and the Rhythm Class Hypothesis. In C. Gussenhoven & N. Warner (Eds.), Laboratory phonology 7 (pp. 515–546). Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Guilbault, C. P. G.
(2002) The acquisition of French rhythm by English second language learners (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Alberta.
Guion, S. G.
(2003) The vowel systems of Quichua-Spanish bilinguals: Age of acquisition effects on the mutual influence of the first and second languages. Phonetica, 60(2), 98–128. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gut, U.
(2010) Cross-linguistic influence in L3 phonological acquisition. International Journal of Multilingualism, 7(1), 19–38. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Han, Q., Tian, J., & Chen, H.
(2023) L3 prosody: Cross-linguistic influence of prosodic features in Mandarin and English by Cantonese multilinguals. In M. M. Brown-Bousfield, S. Flynn, & É. Fernández-Berkes (Eds.), L3 development after the initial state (pp. 96–120). John Benjamins. (this volume). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Izura, C., Cuetos, F., & Brysbaert, M.
(2014) Lextale-Esp: A test to rapidly and efficiently assess the Spanish vocabulary size. Psicológica, 35(1), 49–66.Google Scholar
Lemhöfer, K., & Broersma, M.
(2012) Introducing LexTALE: A quick and valid Lexical Test for Advanced Learners of English. Behavior Research Methods, 44(2), 325–343. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
[URL]
(2020) [URL]
Liu, Z., Gorba, C., & Cebrian, J.
(2019) Effects of learning an additional language on VOT perception. In S. Calhoun, P. Escudero, M. Tabain, & P. Warren (Eds.), Proceedings of the 19th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (pp. 260–264). Australasian Speech Science and Technology Association.Google Scholar
Low, L. E., Grabe, E., & Nolan, F.
(2000) Quantitative characterizations of speech rhythm: Syllable-timing in Singapore English. Language and Speech, 43(4), 377–401. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mayr, R., Sánchez, D., & Mennen, I.
(2020) Does teaching your native language abroad increase L1 attrition of speech? The case of Spaniards in the United Kingdom. Languages, 5(4), 41. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mehler, J., Jusczyk, P., Lambertz, G., Halsted, N., Bertoncini, J., & Amiel-Tison, C.
(1988) A precursor of language acquisition in young infants. Cognition, 29(2), 143–178. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mennen, I.
(2004) Bi-directional interference in the intonation of Dutch speakers of Greek. Journal of Phonetics, 32(4), 543–563. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Minai, U., Gustafson, K., Fiorentino, R., Jongman, A., & Sereno, J.
(2017) Fetal rhythm-based language discrimination: A biomagnetometry study. NeuroReport, 28(10), 561–564. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nazzi, T., Bertoncini, J., & Mehler, J.
(1998) Language discrimination by newborns: Toward an understanding of the role of rhythm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(3), 756–766.Google Scholar
Nespor, M., Shukla, M., & Mehler, J.
(2011) Stress-timed vs. syllable-timed languages. In M. van Oostendorp, C. J. Ewen, E. Hume, & K. Rice (Eds.), The Blackwell companion to phonology: Vol. II, Suprasegmental and prosodic phonology (Chap. 48). Wiley-Blackwell. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Olson, D. J.
(2020) Short-term sources of cross-linguistic phonetic influence: Examining the role of linguistic environment. Languages, 5(4), 43. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ordin, M., & Polyanskaya, L.
(2015a) Acquisition of speech rhythm in a second language by learners with rhythmically different native languages. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 138(2), 533–544. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015b) Perception of speech rhythm in second language: The case of rhythmically similar L1 and L2. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 316. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Polyanskaya, L., & Ordin, M.
(2015) Acquisition of speech rhythm in first language. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 138(3), EL199–EL204. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Puig-Mayenco, E., González Alonso, J., & Rothman, J.
(2018) A systematic review of transfer studies in third language acquisition. Second Language Research, 36(1), 31–64. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Qualtrics
(2021) Qualtrics research suite. [URL]
Ramus, F., Nespor, M., & Mehler, J.
(1999) Correlates of linguistic rhythm in the speech signal. Cognition, 73(3), 265–292. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rothman, J.
(2010) On the typological economy of syntactic transfer: Word order and relative clause high/low attachment preference in L3 Brazilian Portuguese. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 48(2–3), 245–273. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011) L3 syntactic transfer selectivity and typological determinacy: The Typological Primacy Model. Second Language Research, 27(1), 107–127. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2013) Cognitive economy, non-redundancy and typological primacy in L3 acquisition: Evidence from initial stages of L3 Romance. In S. Baauw, F. Drijkoningen, & M. Pinto (Eds.), Romance languages and linguistic theory 2005 (pp. 217–247). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015) Linguistic and cognitive motivations for the Typological Primacy Model (TPM) of third language (L3) transfer: Timing of acquisition and proficiency considered. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(2), 179–190. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rothman, J., & Cabrelli Amaro, J.
(2010) What variables condition syntactic transfer? A look at the L3 initial state. Second Language Research, 26(2), 189–218. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schepens, J., Dijkstra, T., & Grootjen, F.
(2012) Distributions of cognates in Europe as based on Levenshtein distance. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15(1), 157–166. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schepens, J., Dijkstra, T., Grootjen, F., & van Heuven, W.
(2013) Cross-language distributions of high frequency and phonetically similar cognates. PLoS ONE, 8(5), e63006. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schmid, M., & Köpke, B.
(2017) The relevance of first language attrition to theories of bilingual development. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 7(6), 637–667. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, E., & Post, B.
(2015) The development of prosodic features and their contribution to rhythm production in simultaneous bilinguals. Language and Speech, 58(1), 24–47. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sypiańska, J.
(2016) Multilingual acquisition of vowels in L1 Polish, L2 Danish and L3 English. International Journal of Multilingualism, 13(4), 476–495. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ulbrich, C.
(2012) German pitches in English: Production and perception of cross-varietal differences in L2. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16(2), 397–419. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2014) Can L2-English influence L1-German? The case of post-vocalic /r/. Journal of Phonetics, 45, 26–42. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Westergaard, M., Mitrofanova, N., Mykhaylyk, R., & Rodina, Y.
(2017) Crosslinguistic influence in the acquisition of a third language: The Linguistic Proximity Model. International Journal of Bilingualism, 21(6), 666–682. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
White, L., & Mattys, S.
(2007) Calibrating rhythm: First language and second language studies. Journal of Phonetics, 35(4), 501–522. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Willems, N.
(1982) English intonation from a Dutch point of view. Foris. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zhang, L., Morales-Front, A., & Sanz, C.
(2023) Dynamics in cross-linguistic influence – Evidence from VOT development of Mandarin-English bilingual learners of L3 Spanish. In M. M. Brown-Bousfield, S. Flynn, & É. Fernández-Berkes (Eds.), L3 development after the initial state (pp. 121–149). John Benjamins. (this volume). DOI logoGoogle Scholar