References
Adrián, J. A., Alegria, J., & Morais, J.
(1995) Metaphonological abilities of Spanish illiterate adults. International Journal of Psychology, 30(3), 329–351. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Armstrong, A.
(2024) Literacy effects on language acquisition and sentence processing in adult L1 and school-age heritage speakers of Spanish (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). The University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign.
Armstrong, A., & Montrul, S.
(2022) The impact of literacy on the comprehension of verbal passives in school-age Spanish heritage speakers and L1 Spanish adults. Poster presented at the 47th Boston University Conference on Language Development, 3–6 November.
Bayram, F., Kubota, M., Luque, A., Pascual y Cabo, D., & Rothman, J.
(2021) You can’t fix what is not broken: Contextualizing the imbalance of perceptions about heritage language bilingualism. Frontiers in Education, 6, 628311. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Berman, R.
(2008) The psycholinguistics of developing text construction. Journal of Child Language, 35(4), 735–771. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2016) Linguistic literacy and later language development. In J. Perera, M. Aparici, E. Rosado, & N. Salas (Eds.), Written and spoken language development across the lifespan: Essays in honour of Liliana Tolchinsky (pp. 181–200). Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Berman, R., & Ravid, D.
(2009) Becoming a literate language user. In D. Olson & N. Torrance (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of literacy (pp. 92–111). Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Biber, D., & Conrad, S.
(2001) Register variation: A corpus approach. In D. Schiffrin, D. Tannen, & H. E. Hamilton (Eds.), The handbook of discourse analysis (pp. 175–196). Blackwell.Google Scholar
Biber, D., Davies, M., Jones, J. K., & Tracy-Ventura, N.
(2006) Spoken and written variation in Spanish: A multi-dimensional analysis. Corpora, 1(1), 1–37. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bohannon, J. N., & Stanowicz, L. B.
(1988) The issue of negative evidence: Adult responses to children’s language errors. Developmental Psychology, 24(5), 684–689. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Braine, M. D., Brooks, P. J., Cowan, N., Samuels, M. C., & Tamis-LeMonda, C.
(1993) The development of categories at the semantics/syntax interface. Cognitive Development, 8(4), 465–494. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carreira, M., & Kagan, O.
(2011) The results of the National Heritage Language Survey: Implications for teaching, curriculum design, and professional development. Foreign Language Annals, 44(1), 40–64. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carroll, S. E.
(2017) Exposure and input in bilingual development. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20(1), 3–16. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Castro-Calda. A., Petersson. K. M., Reis, A., Stone-Elander, S., & Ingvar, M.
(1998) The illiterate brain. Learning in childhood influences functional organization of adult brain. Brain, 121(6), 1053–1063. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ciapuscio, G. E.
(1992) Impersonalidad y desagentivación en la divulgación científica. Lingüística Española Actual, 14(2), 183–206.Google Scholar
Collier, V. P., & Thomas, W. P.
(2004) The astounding effectiveness of dual language education for all. NABE Journal of Research and Practice, 2(1), 1–20.Google Scholar
(2017) Validating the power of bilingual schooling: Thirty-two years of large-scale, longitudinal research. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 37, 203–217). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Coşkun Kunduz, A. & Montrul, S.
(2022) Sources of variability in the acquisition of Differential Object Marking by Turkish heritage language children in the United States. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 25(4), 603–616. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Coşkun Kunduz, A., & Montrul, S.
(2023) The role of input in the acquisition of evidentiality by Turkish heritage language children in the United States. Language Acquisition. Advance Online Publication.Google Scholar
Crain, S., & Lillo-Martin, D. C.
(1999) An introduction to linguistic theory and language acquisition. The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Cuza, A., Miller, L., Pérez Tattam, R., & Ortiz Vergara, M.
(2019) Structure complexity effects in child heritage Spanish: The case of the Spanish personal a. International Journal of Bilingualism, 23(6), 1333–1357. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dąbrowska, E.
(1997) The LAD goes to school: A cautionary tale for nativists. Linguistics 35(4), 735–766. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2001) Learning a morphological system without a default: The Polish genitive. Journal of Child Language, 28(3), 545–574. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2005) Productivity and beyond: mastering the Polish genitive inflection. Journal of Child Language, 32(1), 191–205. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2006) Low-level schemas or general rules? The role of diminutives in the acquisition of Polish case inflections. Language Sciences, 28(1), 120–135. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2018) Experience, aptitude and individual differences in native language ultimate attainment. Cognition, 178, 222–235. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012) Different speakers, different grammars: Individual differences in native language attainment. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 2(3), 219–253. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2020) How writing changes language. In A. Mauranen & S. Vetchinnikova (Eds.), Language change: The impact of English as a lingua franca (pp. 75–94). Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dąbrowska, E., & Street, J.
(2006) Individual differences in language attainment: Comprehension of passive sentences by native and non-native English speakers. Language Sciences, 28(6), 604–615. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dąbrowska, E., Pascual, E., & Gómez-Estern, B. M.
(2022) Literacy improves the comprehension of object relatives. Cognition, 224, 104958. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Daskalaki, E., Blom, E., Chondrogianni, V., & Paradis, J.
(2020) Effects of parental input quality in child heritage language acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 47(4), 709–736. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
De Houwer, A.
(2009) Bilingual first language acquisition. Multilingual Matters. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ellis, R.
(2005) Measuring implicit and explicit knowledge of a second language: A psychometric study. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27(2), 141–172. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Erlam, R.
(2006) Elicited imitation as a measure of L2 implicit knowledge: An empirical validation study. Applied Linguistics, 27(3), 464–491. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Etxebarria Zuluaga, E.
(2022) Development of pronominal expression in school-age children: Basque-Spanish contact (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign.
Ferreira, F.
(2003) The misinterpretation of noncanonical sentences. Cognitive Psychology, 47(2), 164–203. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fox, D., & Grodzinsky, Y.
(1998) Children’s passive: A view from the by-phrase. Linguistic Inquiry, 29(2), 311–332. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gámez, P. B., & Shimpi, P. M.
(2016) Structural priming in Spanish as evidence of implicit learning. Journal of Child Language, 43, 207–233. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Genesee, F., Lindholm-Leary, K., Saunders, W., & Christian, D.
(2006) Educating English language learners. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T. H., Starr, J., & Ferreira, V. S.
(2015) More than use it or lose it: The number-of-speakers effect on heritage language proficiency. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22(1), 147–155. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Green, J. N.
(1975) On the frequency of passive constructions in Modern Spanish. Bulletin of Hispanic Linguistics, 52(4), 345–362.Google Scholar
Grodzinsky, Y.
(2000) The neurology of syntax: Language use without Broca’s area. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 1–71. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grosjean, F., & Py, B.
(1991) La restructuration d’une première langue: l’Intégration de variantes de contact dans la compétence de migrants bilingues. La Linguistique, 27(2), 35–60.Google Scholar
Guasti, M. T.
(2002) Language acquisition: The growth of grammar. The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Guasti, M. T., & Cardinaletti, A.
(2003) Relative clause formation in Romance child’s production. Probus, 15(1), 47–89. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gutiérrez–Clellen, V. F., & Kreiter, J.
(2003) Understanding child bilingual acquisition using parent and teacher reports. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24(2), 267–288. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hernandez, A. E., Bates, E. A., & Avila, L. X.
(1994) On-line sentence interpretation in Spanish–English bilinguals: What does it mean to be “in between”? Applied Psycholinguistics, 15(4), 417–446. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Holme, R.
(2004) Literacy. Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Huang, B. H., Bedore, L., Niu, L., Wang, Y., & Wicha, N.
(2020) The contributions of oral language to English reading outcomes among young bilingual students in the United States. International Journal of Bilingualism, 25(1), 40–57. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Huettig, F., Singh, N., & Mishra, R. K.
(2011) Language-mediated visual orienting behavior in low and high literates. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 285. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jia, G., & Aaronson, D.
(2003) A longitudinal study of Chinese children and adolescents learning English in the United States. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24(1), 131–161. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jia, R., & Paradis, J.
(2015) The use of referring expressions in narratives by Mandarin heritage language children and the role of language environment factors in predicting individual differences. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(4), 737–752. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kail, R.
(2002) Developmental change in proactive interference. Child Development, 73(6), 1703–1714. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kurvers, J., Vallen, T., & Hout, R. V.
(2006) Discovering features of language: Metalinguisitc awareness of adult illiterates. LOT Occasional Series, 6, 69–88.Google Scholar
MacDonald, M. C., Pearlmutter, N. J., & Seidenberg, M. S.
(1994) The lexical nature of syntactic ambiguity resolution. Psychological Review, 101(4), 676. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mai, Z., Zhao, L., & Yip, V.
(2022) The Mandarin ba-construction in school-age heritage speakers and their parental input. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 12(3–4) DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marinis, T.
(2007) On-line processing of passives in L1 and L2 children. In A. Belikova, L. Meroni, & M. Umeda (Eds.), Proceedings of the 2nd conference on generative approaches to language acquisition North America (GALANA) (pp. 265–276). Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
Marinis, T., & Saddy, D.
(2013) Parsing the passive: Comparing children with specific language impairment to sequential bilingual children. Language Acquisition, 20(2), 155–179. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meisel, J. M.
(2013) Heritage language learners: Unprecedented opportunities for the study of grammars and their development? Theoretical Linguistics, 39(3–4), 225–236. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Menyuk, P., & Brisk, M.
(2005) Language development and education: Children with varying language experiences. Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Merino, B. J.
(1983) Language loss in bilingual Chicano children. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 4(3), 277–294. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Michaels, S., & Collins, I.
(1984) Oral discourse styles: classroom interaction and the acquisition of literacy. In D. Tannen (Ed.), Spoken and written language. Ablex.Google Scholar
Mishra, R. K., Singh, N., Pandey, A., & Huettig, F.
(2012) Spoken language-mediated anticipatory eye movements are modulated by reading ability: Evidence from Indian low and high literates. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 5(1), 1–10. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Montag, J. L., & MacDonald, M. C.
(2015) Text exposure predicts spoken production of complex sentences in 8- and 12-year-old children and adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144(2), 447–468. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2016) The acquisition of heritage languages. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
(2022) Native speakers, interrupted. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S., & Potowski, K.
(2007) Command of gender agreement in school-age Spanish-English bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingualism, 11(3), 301–328. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S., Foote, R., & Perpiñán, S.
(2008) Gender agreement in adult second language learners and Spanish heritage speakers: The effects of age and context of acquisition. Language Learning, 58(3), 503–553. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S., Bhatt, R., & Bhatia, A.
(2012) Erosion of case and agreement in Hindi heritage speakers. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 2(2), 141–176. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mueller Gathercole, V.
(2002a) Grammatical gender in bilingual and monolingual children: A Spanish morphosyntactic distinction. In K. Oller & R. Eilers (Eds.), Language and literacy in bilingual children (pp. 207–219). Multilingual Matters. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2002b) Command of the mass/count distinction in bilingual and monolingual children: An English morphosyntactic distinction. In K. Oller & R. Eilers (Eds.), Language and literacy in bilingual children (pp. 175–208). Multilingual Matters. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2002c) Monolingual and bilingual acquisition: Learning different treatments of that-trace phenomena in English and Spanish. In K. Oller & R. Eilers (Eds.), Language and literacy in bilingual children (pp. 220–254). Multilingual Matters. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nippold, M.
(2016) Later language development. Pro-ed.Google Scholar
Pakulak, E., & Neville, H. J.
(2010) Proficiency differences in syntactic processing of monolingual native speakers indexed by event-related potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(12), 2728–2744. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J., & Genesee, F.
(1996) Syntactic acquisition in bilingual children: Autonomous or interdependent? Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 18(1), 1–25. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1997) On continuity and the emergence of functional categories in bilingual first-language acquisition. Language acquisition, 6(2), 91–124. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Parodi, G.
(Ed.) (2007) Working with Spanish corpora. Continuum.Google Scholar
Pearlmutter, N. J., Garnsey, S. M., & Bock, K.
(1999) Agreement processes in sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 41(3), 427–456. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Petersson, K. M., Reis, A., Askelof, S., Castro-Caldas, A. & Ingvar, M.
(2000) Language processing modulated by literacy: A network analysis of verbal repetition in literate and illiterate subjects. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(3), 364–382. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pires, A., & Rothman, J.
(2009) Disentangling sources of incomplete acquisition: An explanation for competence divergence across heritage grammars. International Journal of Bilingualism, 13(2), 211–238. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ravid, D., & Tolchinsky, L.
(2002) Developing linguistic literacy: A comprehensive model. Journal of Child Language, 29(2), 417–447. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Reis, A., & Castro-Caldas, A.
(1997) Illiteracy: A cause for biased cognitive development. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 3(5), 444–450. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rothman, J.
(2007) Heritage speaker competence differences, language change, and input type: Inflected infinitives in Heritage Brazilian Portuguese. International Journal of Bilingualism, 11(4), 359–389. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sanchez Walker, N.
(2019) Comprehension of Spanish relative and passive clauses by early bilinguals and second language learners (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). The University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign.
Santos, F. H., & Bueno, O. F. A.
(2003) Validation of the Brazilian Children’s Test of Pseudoword Repetition in Portuguese speakers aged 4 to 10 years. Brazilian Journal of Medical and Biological Research, 36(11), 1533–1547. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Seidenberg, M. S., & MacDonald, M. C.
(1999) A probabilistic constraints approach to language acquisition and processing. Cognitive Science, 23(4), 569–588. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Silva-Corvalán, C.
(2014) Bilingual acquisition: Spanish and English in the first six years. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sorace, A.
(2020) L1 attrition in a wider perspective. Second Language Research, 36(2), 203–206. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Street, J. A., & Dąbrowska, E.
(2010) More individual differences in language attainment: How much do adult native speakers of English know about passives and quantifiers? Lingua, 120(8), 2080–2094. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tarone, E., & Bigelow, M.
(2005) Impact of literacy on oral language processing: Implications for second language acquisition research. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 25, 77–97. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Thomas, W. P., & Collier, V. P.
(2002) A National study of school effectiveness for language minority studetns’ long-term academic achievement. Center for Research on Education, Diversity, and Excellence, University of California, Santa Cruz.Google Scholar
Torregrossa, J., Flores, C., & Rinke, E.
(2022) What modulates the acquisition of difficult structures in a heritage language? A study on Portuguese in contact with French, German and Italian. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 26(1), 179–192. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ud Deen, K.
(2011) The acquisition of the passive. In J. de Villiers & T. Roeper (Eds.), Handbook of generative approaches to language acquisition (pp. 155–187). Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Unsworth, S.
(2013) Assessing the role of current and cumulative exposure in simultaneous bilingual acquisition: The case of Dutch gender. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16(1), 86–110. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Osch, B., Hulk, A., Sleeman, P., & van Suchtelen, P. I.