Paradigmatic consequences of the suffixing preference
As one of the most robust typological findings, the suffixing preference captures the empirical observation that grammatical categories are more likely to be coded by suffixes than by prefixes. The goal of this contribution is to explore the effects that this asymmetry may have on the inflectional paradigms of the languages of the world. Three empirical issues are addressed: do languages with either possessive prefixes or suffixes and languages with both possessive prefixes and suffixes differ in their suffix-to-prefix ratio? Do prefixes and suffixes differ in the number of cases that they code? Do prefixes and suffixes differ in their probability of explicit singular in addition to plural marking? The answer to all three questions is in the affirmative. These effects are understood in terms of a response to an inherent disadvantage of prefixes. Morphological systems reduce the processing difficulty incurred by prefixes by assigning them fewer tasks (i.e. number of cases), by limiting their occurrence in highly competitive contexts (i.e. inconsistent possessive-affix coding) and by creating prefix paradigms, which are conceived of as protective structures in which the individual members strengthen one another. The general claim these three effects lead up to is that morphological systems develop “repair strategies” which reduce the processing cost involved in using inherently disadvantaged units. These repair strategies shape morphological structure.
Article outline
- 1.Introduction
- 2.Data analysis
- 2.1Inconsistent possessor marking
- 2.2Number of nominal cases
- 2.3Paradigms in nominal-number marking
- 3.Theoretical discussion
- 3.1The (in)consistency of paradigms
- 3.2The size of paradigms
- 3.3The presence or absence of paradigms
- 3.4Predictions
- 4.Synthesis
- Acknowledgements
- Notes
- Abbreviations
-
References
References (53)
References
Alungum, John, Robert J. Conrad & Joshua Lukas. 1978. Some Muhian grammatical notes. In Richard Loving (ed.), Miscellaneous papers on Dobu and Arapesh, 89–130. Ukarumpa: Summer Institute of Linguistics.
Asao, Yoshihiko. 2015. Left-right asymmetries in words: A processing-based account. Buffalo: State University of New York at Buffalo PhD dissertation.
Bauer, Laurie. 2019. Notions of paradigm and their value in word-formation. Word Structure 121. 153–175.
Berg, Thomas. 2016. The multiplanar nature of frequency. Glottotheory 71. 1–19.
Berg, Thomas. 2020. Ordering biases in cross-linguistic perspective: The interaction of linear order and structural level. Linguistic Typology 241. 353–397.
Boas, Franz. 1911. Chinook. In Franz Boas (ed.), Handbook of American Indian languages. Vol. 11, 559–665. Washington: Government Printing Office.
Bock, Kathryn & Carol A. Miller. 1991. Broken agreement. Cognitive Psychology 231. 45–93.
Bodian, Lamine. 2017. Morphosyntaxe du guñaamolo, parler baïnounk de Niamone. Munich: Lincom.
Bouquiaux, Luc. 1970. La langue Birom. Paris: Société d’Editions “Les Belles Lettres”.
Briggs, Elinor. 1961. Mitla Zapotec grammar. México: Instituto Lingüístico de Verano.
Bunn, Gordon. 1974. Golin grammar. Ukarumpa: Summer Institute of Linguistics.
Cabral, Ana Suelly de Arruda Camara. 1995. Contact-induced language change in the Western Amazon: The non-genetic origin of the Kokama language. Pittsburgh: University of Pittsburgh PhD dissertation.
Casad, Eugene H. 1984. Cora. In Ronald W. Langacker (ed.), Studies in Uto-Aztecan grammar. Vol. 41, 151–459. Dallas, TX: Summer Institute of Linguistics.
Collins, Chris & Levi Namaseb. 2011. A grammatical sketch of N|uuki with stories. Cologne: Köppe.
Cowan, H. K. J. 1955. Notes on Windesi grammar. Oceania 261. 42–58.
Crowell, Thomas H. 1979. A grammar of Bororo. Ithaca: Cornell University PhD dissertation.
Cysouw, Michael. 2003. The paradigmatic structure of person marking. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Dryer, Matthew S. 2013a. Prefixing vs. suffixing in inflectional morphology. In Matthew S. Dryer & Martin Haspelmath (eds.), The world atlas of language structures online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. Available at: [URL] (last access 22 June 2021).
Dryer, Matthew S. 2013b. Position of case affixes. In Matthew S. Dryer & Martin Haspelmath (eds.), The world atlas of language structures online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. Available at: [URL] (last access 22 June 2021).
Dryer, Matthew S. 2013c. Position of pronominal possessive affixes. In Matthew S. Dryer & Martin Haspelmath (eds.), The world atlas of language structures online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. Available at: [URL] (last access 22 June 2021).
Einarsson, Stefán. 1945. Icelandic. Baltimore: John Hopkins University Press.
Ferreira, Victor S. & Kathryn Bock. 2006. The functions of structural priming. Language and Cognitive Processes 211. 1011–1029.
Gallagher, Steve & Pierce Baehr. 2005. Bariai grammar sketch (Data papers on Papua New Guinea Languages 49). Ukarumpa: Summer Institute of Linguistics.
Glushko, Robert J. 1979. The organization and activation of orthographic knowledge in reading aloud. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 51. 674–691.
Green, Rebecca. 1995. A grammar of Gurr-goni (North Central Arnhem Land). Canberra: The Australian National University PhD dissertation.
Greenberg, Joseph H. 1963. Some universals of grammar with particular reference to the order of meaningful elements. In Joseph H. Greenberg (ed.), Universals of language, 73–113. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Haspelmath, Martin, Matthew S. Dryer, David Gil & Bernard Comrie. 2005. The world atlas of language structures. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Haspelmath, Martin & Andres Karjus. 2017. Explaining asymmetries in number marking: singulatives, pluratives, and usage frequency. Linguistics 551. 1213–1235.
Hercus, Luise. 1999. A grammar of the Wirangu language from the west coast of South Australia. Canberra: The Australian National University.
Iggesen, Oliver A. 2013. Number of cases. In: Matthew S. Dryer & Martin Haspelmath (eds.), The world atlas of language structures online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. Available at: [URL] (last access 22 June 2021).
Jany, Carmen. 2009. Chimariko grammar. Areal and typological perspective. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Kähler, Hans. 1940. Grammatischer Abriss des Enggano. Zeitschrift für Eingeborenensprachen 301. 81–320.
MacKay, Donald G. 1987. The organization of perception and action. New York: Springer.
Merlan, Francesca. 1983. Ngalakan grammar, texts and vocabulary. Canberra: The Australian National University.
Mitchell, David B. & Alan S. Brown. 1988. Persistent repetition priming in picture naming and its dissociation from recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 141. 213–222.
Nordlinger, Rachel & Louisa Sadler. 2004. Nominal tense in crosslinguistic perspective. Language 801. 776–806.
Santandrea, Stefano. 1963. A concise grammar outline of the Bongo language. Rome: St. Peter Claver.
Sapir, Edward. 1921. Language. New York: Harcourt, Brace & Co.
Saxton, Dean. 1982. Papago. In Ronald W. Langacker (ed.), Studies in Uto-Aztecan grammar. Vol. 31, 93–266. Dallas, TX: Summer Institute of Linguistics.
Scott, Graham. 1978. The Fore language of Papua New Guinea. Canberra: The Australian National University.
Siewierska, Anna. 2010. Person asymmetries in zero expression and grammatical function. In Franck Floricic (ed.), Essais de typologie et de linguistique générale, 471–485. Lyon: Ecole Normale Supérieure de Lyon.
Štekauer, Pavol, Salvador Valera & Lívia Körtvélyessy. 2012. Word-formation in the world’s languages. A typological survey. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Stemberger, Joseph P. 1991. Apparent anti-frequency effects in language production: The Addition Bias and phonological underspecification. Journal of Memory and Language 301. 161–185.
Taylor, Insup. 1969. Content and structure in sentence production. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 81. 170–175.
Tharp, Doug. 1996. Sulka grammar essentials. In John M. Clifton (ed.), Two non-Austronesian grammars from the islands, 77–179. Ukarumpa: Summer Institute of Linguistics.
Topping, Donald M. 1973. Chamorro reference grammar. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press.
Tucker, Archibald N. & Margaret A. Bryan. 1966. Linguistic analyses. The non-Bantu languages of North-eastern Africa. London: Oxford University Press.
Vogt, Hans. 1940. The Kalispel language. Oslo: I Kommisjon hos Jacob Dybwad.
Waterhouse, Viola. 1967. Huamelultec Chontal. In Robert Wauchope (ed.), Handbook of Middle American Indians, Vol. 5: Linguistics, 349–367. Austin, TX: University of Texas Press.
Cited by (2)
Cited by two other publications
Herce, Borja
2024.
VeLePa: Central Pame verbal inflection in a quantitative perspective.
Morphology 34:3
► pp. 281 ff.
Zingler, Tim
2023.
A functional approach to the formal mismatches of indexation markers.
Zeitschrift für Sprachwissenschaft 42:2
► pp. 373 ff.
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 24 september 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.