Part of
Essays on Linguistic Realism
Edited by Christina Behme and Martin Neef
[Studies in Language Companion Series 196] 2018
► pp. 6178
References (69)
References
Arbib, Michael A. 2005. From monkey-like action recognition to human language: An evolutionary framework for neurolinguistics. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28: 105–167. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Behme, Christina. 2015. Is the ontology of biolinguistics coherent? Language Sciences 47: 32–42. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Berwick, Robert C., Pietroski, Paul, Yankama, Beracah & Chomsky, Noam. 2011. Poverty of the stimulus revisited. Cognitive Science 35: 1207–1242. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bickerton, Derek. 1981. Roots of Language. Ann Arbor MI: Karoma.Google Scholar
Bugnyar, Thomas & Heinrich, Bernd. 2005. Food-storing ravens differentiate between knowledgeable and ignorant competitors. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B 272: 1641–1646. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Burling, Robbins. 1986. The selective advantage of complex language. Ethology and Sociobiology 7: 1–16. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chen, Iani, Jansen, Naomi & ten Cate, Carel. 2014. Zebra finches can learn to recognize affixations. In The Evolution of Language: Proceedings of the 10th International Conference EVOLANG 10, Erica C. Cartmill, Seán Roberts, Heid Lyn & Hannah Cornish (eds), 411–412. Singapore: World Scientific. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1975. Reflections on Language. London: Temple Smith.Google Scholar
. 1977. Essays on Form and Interpretation. New York NY: North Holland.Google Scholar
. 1980. The linguistic approach. In Language and Learning, Massimo Piatelli-Palmerini (ed.), 107–130. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
. 1986. Knowledge of Language. New York NY: Praeger.Google Scholar
. 2009. Opening remarks. In Of Minds and Language: A Dialogue with Noam Chomsky in the Basque Country, Massimo Piattelli-Palmarini, Juan Uriagereka & Pello Salaburu (eds), 13–43. Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
. 2012. The Science of Language. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, Eve. 2009. First Language Acquisition. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Collins, Chris & Postal, Paul M.. 2012. Imposters. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Collins, John. 2009. A question of irresponsibility: Postal, and Chomsky, Gödel. Biolinguistics 3: 99–103.Google Scholar
Corballis, Michael C. 2007. Recursion, language, and starlings. Cognitive Science 31: 697–704. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Curme, George. 1931. Syntax. Boston MA: D. C. Heath.Google Scholar
Deacon, Terrence. 1997. The Symbolic Species. New York NY: W.W. Norton.Google Scholar
. 2009. Relaxed selection and the role of epigenesis in the evolution of language. In Oxford Handbook of Development Behavioral Neuroscience, Mark S. Blumberg, John H. Freeman & Scott R. Robinson (eds), 730–752. Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
Emery, Nathan J. & Clayton, Nicola S. 2001. Effects of experience and social context on prospective caching strategies by scrub jays. Nature 414: 443–446. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Evans, Nicholas. 2005. Australian languages reconsidered: A review of Dixon (2002). Oceanic Linguistics 44(1): 242–286. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Evans, Nicholas & Levinson, Stephen. 2009. The myth of language universals: Language diversity and its importance for cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32: 429–448. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Evans, Vyvyan. 2014. The Language Myth: Why Language is not an Instinct. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Everett, Daniel. 2012. Language: The Cultural Tool. New York NY: Pantheon.Google Scholar
Fitch, W. Tecumseh. 2010. The Evolution of Language. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fitch, W. Tecumseh & Friederici, Angelika D. 2012. Artificial grammar learning meets formal language theory: An overview. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences 367: 1933–1955. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fodor, Jerrold. 1985. Some notes on what linguistics is about. In The Philosophy of Linguistics, Jerrold J. Katz (ed.), 146–160. Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
Gentner, Timothy Q., Fenn, Kimberly M., Margoliash, Daniel & Nusbaum, Howard C. 2006. Recursive syntactic pattern learning by songbirds. Nature 440: 1204–1207. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Genty, Emilie, Clay, Zanna, Hobaiter, Catherine & Zuberbuehler, Klaus. 2013. Multi-modal use of a socially directed call in bonobos. PLoS ONE 9(1): e84738.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Heim, Irene. 2008. Features on bound pronouns. In Phi-theory: Phi-features across modules and interfaces, Daniel Harbour, David Adger & Susana Bejar (eds), 35–56. Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
Hurford, James. 1989. Biological evolution of the Saussurean sign as a component of the language acquisition device. Lingua 77(2): 187–222. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 2007. The Origins of Meaning: Language in the Light of Evolution. Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
. 2011. The Origins of Grammar: Language in the Light of Evolution. Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
. 2014. The Origins of Language: A Slim Guide. Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
Iten, Corinne, Stainton, Robert & Wearing, Catherine. 2007. On restricting the evidence base for linguistics. In Philosophy of Psychology, Paul Thagard, Dov Gabbay & John Woods (eds), 219–246. Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, Ray & Pinker, Steven. 2005. The nature of the language faculty and its implications for evolution of language (reply to Fitch, Hauser, and Chomsky). Cognition 97: 211–225. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Janik, Vincent M. 2014. Cetacean vocal learning and communication. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 28: 60–65. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kanwal, Jagmeet S., Matsumura, Sumiko, Ohlemiller, Kevin & Suga, Nobuo. 1994. Analysis of acoustic elements and syntax in communication sounds emitted by mustached bats. Journal of the Acoustic Society of America 96: 1229–1254. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Katz, Jerrold J. 1981. Language and Other Abstract Objects. Totowa NJ: Rowman and Littlefield.Google Scholar
1996. The unfinished Chomskyan revolution. Mind and Language 11: 270–294. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
1998. Realistic Rationalism. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Katz, Jerrold J. & Postal, Paul M.. 1991. Realism vs. conceptualism in linguistics. Linguistics and Philosophy 14: 515–554. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lieberman, Philip. 2013. The Unpredictable Species. Princeton NJ: Princeton University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ludlow, Peter. 2011. The Philosophy of Generative Linguistics. Oxford: OUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, Brian. 2004. A multiple process solution to the logical problem of language acquisition. Journal of Child Language 31: 883–914. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McGilvray, James. 2006. On the innateness of language. In Contemporary Debates in Cognitive Science, Robert Stainton (ed.), 97–112. Malden MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
. 2009. Introduction to the third edition. In Cartesian Linguistics. A Chapter in the History of Rationalist Thought, Noam Chomsky, 1–52. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 2012. Appendix VI: Semantics and how to do it. In The Science of Language: Interviews with James McGilvray, Noam Chomsky, 206–231. Cambridge: CUP.Google Scholar
Pepperberg, Irene M. 2008. Alex & Me. New York NY: Harper Luxe.Google Scholar
Pinker, Steven. 1994. The Language Instinct. New York NY: William Morrow.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pinker, Steven & Bloom, Paul. 1990. Natural language and natural selection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13: 707–784. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Postal, Paul M. 2009. The incoherence of Chomsky’s ‘biolinguistic’ ontology. Biolinguistics 3: 104–123.Google Scholar
Quine, Willard van Orman. 1987. Quiddities. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Reali, Florencia K. & Christiansen, Morten H. 2005. Uncovering the richness of the stimulus: Structural dependence and indirect statistical evidence. Cognitive Science 29: 1007–1028. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Savage-Rumbaugh, Sue & Lewin, Roger. 1994. Kanzi: The Ape at the Brink of the Human Mind. New York NY: WileyGoogle Scholar
Schel, Anne Marijke, Townsend, Simon W., Machanda, Zarin, Zuberbühler, Klaus & Slocombe, Katie E. 2013. Chimpanzee alarm call production meets key criteria for intentionality. PLoS ONE 8(10): e76674. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schlenker Philippe, Chemla, Emmanuel, Arnold, Kate, Lemasson, Alban, Ouattara, Karim, Keenan, Sumir, Stephan, Claudia, Ryder, Robin & Zuberbühler, Klaus. 2014. Monkey semantics: Two ‘dialects’ of Campbell’s monkey alarm calls. Linguistics and Philosophy 37: 439–501. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schnelle, Helmut. 2010. Language in the Brain. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Searle, John. 1973. Chomsky’s revolution in linguistics. In On Noam Chomsky: Critical essays, Gilman Harman (ed.), 2–33. New York NY: Anchor Books.Google Scholar
Seyfarth, Robert M., Cheney, Dorothy L. & Marler, Peter. 1980. Monkey responses to three different alarm calls: Evidence of predator classification and semantic communication. Science 210(4471): 801–803. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smith, Neil. 1999. Chomsky. Ideas and Ideals. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stainton, Robert. 2006. Words and Thought. Oxford: Clarendon Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Suzuki, Ryuji, Buck, John R. & Tyack, Peter L. 2006. Information entropy of humpback whale songs. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 119(3): 1849–1866. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Taylor, Alexander H., Miller, Rachael & Gray, Russel D. 2012. New Caledonian crows reason about hidden causal agents. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science U S A 109(40): 16389–16391. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
ten Cate, Carel. 2014. On the phonetic and syntactic abilities of birds. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 28: 157–164. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, Michael. 2003. Constructing a Language: A Usage-Based Theory of Language Acquisition. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
. 2009. Origins of Communication. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Watumull, Jeffrey. 2014. Biolinguistics and platonism: Contradictory or consilient? Biolinguistics 7: 301–315.Google Scholar