Article published In:
Sign Language & Linguistics
Vol. 17:1 (2014) ► pp.119
References
Barner, David, Neon Brooks & Alan Bale.
2010Accessing the unsaid: the role of scalar alternatives in children’s pragmatic inference. Cognition 118(1). 84-93. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bott, Lewis & Ira Noveck.
2004Some utterances are underinformative: the onset and time course of scalar inferences. Journal of Memory and Language 51(3). 437-457. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Büring, Daniel.
2003On D-trees, beans, and B-accents. Linguistics and Philosophy 26(5). 511-545. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chierchia, Gennaro, Stephen Crain, Maria Teresa Guasti, Andrea Gualmini & Luisa Meroni.
2001The Acquisition of disjunction: Evidence for a grammatical view of scalar implicatures. In Anna H.-J. Do, Laura Domínguez & Aimee Johansen (eds.), BUCLD 25: Proceedings of the 25th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, 157–168. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
De Neys, Wim & Walter Schaeken.
2007When people are more logical under cognitive load. Experimental Psychology 54(2). 128-133. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Emmorey, Karen (ed.).
2003Perspectives on classifier constructions in sign languages. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gazdar, Gerald.
1979Pragmatics: implicature, presupposition and logical form. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Grice, H. Paul.
1967, revised and published 1989. Studies in the way of words. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Horn, Laurence.
1989A natural history of negation. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Katsos, Napoleon & Dorothy Bishop.
2011Pragmatic tolerance: implications for the acquisition of informativeness and implicature. Cognition 120(1). 67-81. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Noveck, Ira.
2001When children are more logical than adults: experimental investigations of scalar implicature. Cognition 78(2). 165-188. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Papafragou, Anna & Julien Musolino.
2003Scalar implicatures: experiments at the semantics-pragmatics interface. Cognition 86(3). 253-282. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roberts, Craige.
1998Information structure in discourse: Towards an integrated formal theory of pragmatics. Ohio State Working Papers in Linguistics, Columbus, OH.
Sandler, Wendy & Diane Lillo-Martin.
2006Sign language and linguistic universals. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stiller, Alex, Noah Goodman & Michael Frank.
2011Ad hoc scalar implicatures in adults and children. In Laura Carlson, Christopher Hoelscher & Thomas F. Shipley (eds.), Proceedings of the 33rd Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 2134–2139. Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
Storto, Gianluca & Michael Tanenhaus.
2005Are scalar implicatures computed online? In Emar Maeir, Corien Bary & Janneke Huitink (eds.), Proceedings of Sinn und Bedeutung 9, 431-445. Nijmegen: Nijmegen Centre for Semantics.Google Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 3 other publications

Frederiksen, Anne Therese & Rachel I. Mayberry
2016. Who's on First? Investigating the referential hierarchy in simple native ASL narratives. Lingua 180  pp. 49 ff. DOI logo
Kimmelman, Vadim
2021. Acceptability Judgments in Sign Linguistics. In The Cambridge Handbook of Experimental Syntax,  pp. 561 ff. DOI logo
Schlenker, Philippe & Jonathan Lamberton
2022. Meaningful Blurs: the sources of repetition-based plurals in ASL. Linguistics and Philosophy 45:2  pp. 201 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 14 april 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.