Part of
Requesting in Social Interaction
Edited by Paul Drew and Elizabeth Couper-Kuhlen
[Studies in Language and Social Interaction 26] 2014
► pp. 335364
References (48)
References
Austin, John Langshaw. 1962. How to Do Things with Words. Oxford: Clarendon.Google Scholar
Blurton Jones, Nicholas G. 1984. “A Selfish Origin for Human Food Sharing: Tolerated Theft.” Ethology and Sociobiology 5 (1): 1–3. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 1987. “Tolerated Theft, Suggestions about the Ecology and Evolution of Sharing, Hoarding, and Scrounging.” Social Science Information 26 (1): 31–54. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bruner, Jerome. 1975. “The Ontogenesis of Speech Acts.” Journal of Child Language 2: 1–19. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Call, Josep, and Michael Tomasello (eds). 2007. The Gestural Communication of Apes and Monkeys. New York: LEA.Google Scholar
Casasanto, Daniel. 2008. “Who's Afraid of the Big Bad Whorf? Crosslinguistic Differences in Temporal Language and Thought.” Language Learning 58 (S1): 63–79. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cheney, Dorothy L., and Robert M. Seyfarth. 2005. “Constraints and Preadaptations in the Earliest Stages of Language Evolution.” The Linguistic Review 22 (2–4): 135–159. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Corballis, Michael C. 2002. From Hand to Mouth: The Origins of Language. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Curl, Traci, and Paul Drew. 2008. “Contingency and Action: A Comparison of Two Forms of Requesting.” Research on Language and Social Interaction 41 (2): 129–153. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Davidson, Donald. 1982. “Rational Animals.” Dialectica 36 (4): 317–327. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
de Waal, Frans B.M. 1989. “Food Sharing and Reciprocal Obligations among Chimpanzees.” Journal of Human Evolution 18 (5): 433–459. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fedurek, Pawel, and Katie E. Slocombe. 2011. “ Primate Vocal Communication: A Useful Tool for Understanding Human Speech and Language Evolution?” Human Biology 83 (2): 153–173. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fehr, Ernst, Helen Bernhard, and Bettina Rockenbach. 2008. “Egalitarianism in Young Children.” Nature 454 (7208): 1079–1083. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fruth, Barbara, and Gottfried Hohmann. 2002. “How Bonobos Handle Hunts and Harvests: Why Share Food?” In Behavioural Diversity in Chimpanzees and Bonobos, ed. by Christophe Boesch, Gottfried Hohmann, and Linda Marchant, 138–150. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gilby, Ian C., Melissa Emery Thompson, Jonathan D. Ruane, and Richard Wrangham. 2010. “No Evidence of Short-term Exchange of Meat for Sex among Chimpanzees.” Journal of Human Evolution 59 (1): 44–53. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gomes, Cristina M., and Christophe Boesch. 2009. “Wild Chimpanzees Exchange Meat for Sex on a Long-term Basis.” PLoS One 4 (4): e5116. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 2011. “Reciprocity and Trades in Wild West African Chimpanzees.” Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 65 (11): 2183–2196. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gumperz, John H., and Stephen C. Levinson (eds). 1996. Rethinking Linguistic Relativity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Halina, Marta, Federico Rossano, and Michael Tomasello. 2013. “The Ontogenetic Ritualization of Bonobo Gestures.” Animal Cognition 16 (4): 653–666. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hockings, Kimberley J., Tatyana Humle, James R. Anderson, Dora Biro, Claudia Sousa, Gaku Ohashi, and Tetsuro Matsuzawa. 2007. “Chimpanzees Share Forbidden Fruit.” PLoS One 2 (9): e886. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jaeggi, Adrian V., Maria A. Van Noordwijk, and Carel P. Van Schaik. 2008. “Begging for Information: Mother–offspring Food Sharing among Wild Bornean Orangutans.” American Journal of Primatology 70 (6): 533–541. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jaeggi, Adrian V., and Carel P. Van Schaik. 2011. “The Evolution of Food Sharing in Primates.” Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 65 (11): 2125–2140. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kendon, Adam. 1977. “Spatial Organization in Social Encounters: The F-formation System.” In Studies in the Behavior of Social Interaction, ed. by Adam Kendon, 179–208. Lisse, Holland: Peter DeRidder Press.Google Scholar
Leavens, David A., Jamie L. Russell, and William D. Hopkins. 2010. “Multimodal Communication by Captive Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes).” Animal Cognition 13 (1): 33–40. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Levinson, Stephen C. 2006. “On the Human "Interaction Engine".” In Roots of Human Sociality: Culture, Cognition and Interaction, ed. by Nicholas J. Enfield, and Stephen C. Levinson, 39–69. Oxford, UK: Berg.Google Scholar
Liebal, Katja, Simone Pika, and Michael Tomasello. 2006. “Gestural Communication of Orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus).” Gesture 6 (1): 1–38. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liebal, Katja, Bridget M. Waller, Katie E. Slocombe, and Anne M. Burrows. 2013. Primate Communication: A Multimodal Approach. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nishida, Toshisada, and Linda A. Turner. 1996. “Food Transfer between Mother and Infant Chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania.” International Journal of Primatology 17 (6): 947–958. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Partan, Sarah, and Peter Marler. 1999. “Communication Goes Multimodal.” Science 283 (5406): 1272–1273. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rheingold, Harriet L., Dale F. Hay, and Meredith J. West. 1976. “Sharing in the Second Year of Life.” Child Development 47: 1148–1158. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rijksen, Herman Dirk. 1978. A Field Study on Sumatran Orang Utans (Pongo pygmaeus abelii, Lesson 1827): Ecology, Behaviour and Conservation. Netherlands: H. Veenman.Google Scholar
Rossano, Federico. 2013. “Sequence Organization and Timing of Bonobo Mother-infant Interactions.” Interaction Studies 14 (2): 160–189. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sacks, Harvey. 1992 [1964–1972]. Lectures on Conversation, 2 Vols. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Sapir, Edward. 1921. Language: An Introduction to the Study of Speech. New York: Harcourt, Brace and Company.Google Scholar
Schegloff, Emanuel A. 1988. “On an Actual Virtual Servo-Mechanism for Guessing Bad News: A Single Case Conjecture.” Social Problems 35 (4): 442–457. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 2007. Sequence Organization in Interaction: A Primer in Conversation Analysis. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Searle, John R. 1969. Speech Acts: An Essay in the Philosophy of Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Silk, Joan B. 1978. “Patterns of Food Sharing among Mother and Infant Chimpanzees at Gombe National Park, Tanzania.” Folia Primatologica 29: 129–141. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stevens, Jeffrey R., and Ian C. Gilby. 2004. “A Conceptual Framework for Nonkin Food Sharing: Timing and Currency of Benefits.” Animal Behaviour 67 (4): 603–614. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stivers, Tanya, and Federico Rossano. 2010. “Mobilizing Response.” Research on Language and Social Interaction 43 (1): 3–31. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Taglialatela, Jared P., Jamie L. Russell, Jennifer A. Schaeffer, and William D. Hopkins. 2011. “Chimpanzee Vocal Signaling Points to a Multimodal Origin of Human Language.” PLoS One 6 (4): e18852. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, Michael. 2000. “Two Hypotheses about Primate Cognition.” In The Evolution of Cognition, ed. by Cecilia Heyes, and Ludwig Huber, 165–183. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
. 2006. “Why Don't Apes Point?” In Roots of Human Sociality: Culture, Cognition and Interaction, ed. by Nicholas J. Enfield, and Stephen C. Levinson, 506–524. London: Berg.Google Scholar
. 2008. Origins of Human Communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, Michael, and Felix Warneken. 2008. “Human Behaviour: Share and Share Alike.” Nature 454 (7208): 1057–1058. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van Noordwijk, Maria A., and Carel P. van Schaik. 2009. “Intersexual Food Transfer among Orangutans: Do Females Test Males for Coercive Tendency?” Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 63 (6): 883–890. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Whorf, Benjamin. 1956. Language, Thought, and Reality. Cambridge, MA.: M.I.T. Press.Google Scholar
Zuberbühler, Klaus. 2005. “The Phylogenetic Roots of Language: Evidence from Primate Communication and Cognition.” Current Directions in Psychological Science 14 (3): 126–130. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by (9)

Cited by nine other publications

Benetti, Stefania, Ambra Ferrari & Francesco Pavani
2023. Multimodal processing in face-to-face interactions: A bridging link between psycholinguistics and sensory neuroscience. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 17 DOI logo
Fröhlich, Marlen & Carel P. van Schaik
2022. Social tolerance and interactional opportunities as drivers of gestural redoings in orang-utans. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 377:1859 DOI logo
Mondada, Lorenza & Adrien Meguerditchian
2022. Ouvertures et salutations entre babouins : organisation de la séquence et orientation incarnée vers l’autre. Langage et société N° 176:2  pp. 127 ff. DOI logo
Mondémé, Chloé
2022. Why study turn‐taking sequences in interspecies interactions?. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 52:1  pp. 67 ff. DOI logo
Fröhlich, Marlen, Natasha Bartolotta, Caroline Fryns, Colin Wagner, Laurene Momon, Marvin Jaffrezic, Tatang Mitra Setia, Maria A. van Noordwijk & Carel P. van Schaik
2021. Multicomponent and multisensory communicative acts in orang-utans may serve different functions. Communications Biology 4:1 DOI logo
Rauniomaa, Mirka, Tiina Keisanen & Pauliina Siitonen
Urbanik, Paweł
2020. Getting others to share goods in Polish and Norwegian: Material and moral anchors for request conventions. Intercultural Pragmatics 17:2  pp. 177 ff. DOI logo
Holler, Judith & Stephen C. Levinson
2019. Multimodal Language Processing in Human Communication. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 23:8  pp. 639 ff. DOI logo
Kanngiesser, Patricia, Federico Rossano & Michael Tomasello
2015. Late Emergence of the First Possession Heuristic: Evidence From a Small‐Scale Culture. Child Development 86:4  pp. 1282 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 5 november 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.