Fiction enables readers to simulate the social experiences of characters and may facilitate prosociality. Research
has indicated that fiction print exposure positively relates to empathy and may promote altruistic behaviors. Whether associations
hold across different media formats and thematic genres remains unclear. This study took a multidimensional approach to both
fiction engagement and empathic abilities. Specifically, it aimed to replicate previous findings that lifetime fiction exposure
positively predicts empathy, and to extend this literature through an exploration of the relationships between media and genre
formats, empathy and altruism. Participants (N = 404) completed a multidimensional task measure of fiction media
exposure and answered questions about fiction engagement, empathic and altruistic tendencies. Results showed divergent
associations between fiction format, genre, and empathic abilities, and fiction media exposure positively predicted the tendencies
to become transported into narratives and to help others. Engagement with fiction formats and thematic genres may differentially
support adults’ prosocial development.
Acheson, D. J., Wells, J. B., & MacDonald, M. C. (2008). New and updated tests of print exposure and reading abilities in college students. Behavior Research Methods, 401, 278–289.
Appel, M., & Richter, T. (2007). Persuasive effects of fictional narratives increase over time. Media Psychology, 101, 113–134.
Aykan, S., & Nalçacı, E. (2018). Assessing theory of mind by humor: The Humor Comprehension and Appreciation Test (ToM-HCAT). Frontiers in Psychology, 91, 1470.
Bal, P. M. & Veltkamp, M. (2013). How does fiction reading influence empathy? An experimental investigation of the role of emotional transportation. PLoSOne, 8(1),
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I. (2001). The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” Test revised version: a study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 421, 241–251.
Baron-Cohen, S. & Wheelwright, S. J. (2004). The empathy quotient. An investigation of adults with Asperger Syndrome or High Functioning Autism, and normal sex differences. Autism and Developmental Disorders, 341, 163–175.
Batson, C. D., Duncan, B. D., Ackerman, P., Buckley, T. & Birch, K. (1981). Is empathic emotion a source of altruistic motivation?Personality and Social Psychology, 401, 290–302.
Batson, C. D., Early, S. & Salvarani, G. (1997). Perspective-taking: Imagining how another would feel versus imagining how you would feel. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 231, 751–758.
Batson, C. D., & Shaw, L. (1991). Evidence for altruism: Toward a pluralism of prosocial motives. Psychological Inquiry, 21, 107–122.
Black, J. E., & Barnes, J. L. (2015a). Fiction and social cognition: The effect of viewing award-winning television dramas on theory of mind. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 91, 355–494.
Black, J. E., & Barnes, J. L. (2015b). The effects of reading material on social and non-social cognition. Poetics, 521, 32–43.
Black, J. E., Capps, S. C., & Barnes, J. L. (2018). Fiction, genre exposure and moral reality. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity and the Arts, 12(13), 328–340.
Bormann, D., & Greitemeyer, T. (2015). Immersed in virtual worlds and minds: effects of in-game storytelling on immersion, need satisfaction, and affective theory of mind. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 61, 646–652.
Bureau of Labor Statistics (2017). American Time Use Survey – 2016 Results. Retrieved from: [URL]
Busselle, R., & Bilandzic, H. (2009). Measuring narrative engagement. Media Psychology, 121, 321–347.
Castano, E. (2012). Anti-social behavior in individuals and groups: an empathy-focused approach. In K. Deux & M. Snyder (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of personality and social psychology (pp. 419–445). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Coll, M., Viding, E., Rütgen, M., Silani, G., Lamm, C., Catmur, C. & Bird, G. (2017). Are we really measuring empathy? Proposal for a new measurement framework. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 831, 132–139.
Davis, M. H. (1980). A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. JSAS Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology, 101, 85. Retrieved from: [URL]
Davis, M. H., Mitchell, K. V., Hall, J. A., Lothert, J., Snapp, T. & Meyer, M. (1999). Empathy, expectations, and situational preferences: Personality influences on the decision to participate in volunteer helping behaviors. Personality, 671, 469–503.
Djikic, M. & Oatley, K. (2014). The art in fiction: From indirect communication to changes in the self, Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity and the Arts, 81, 498–505.
Dodell-Feder, D. & Tamir, D. I. (2018). Fiction reading has a small positive impact on social cognition: A meta-analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 1471, 1713–1727.
Duval, C., Piolino, P., Bejanin, A., Eustache, F., & Desgranges, B. (2010). Age effects on different components of theory of mind. Consciousness and Cognition 201, 627–642.
Felisberti, F. M., & King, R. (2017). Mindreading in altruists and psychopaths. In A. Ibáñez, L. Sedeño & A. García (Eds.), Neuroscience and social science: The missing link (pp. 121–140). Cham, Switzerland: Springer.
Fong, K., Mullin, J. B., & Mar, R. A. (2013). What you read matters: The role of fiction genre in predicting interpersonal sensitivity. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity and the Arts, 71, 370–376.
Gabriel, S., & Young, A. F. (2011). Becoming a vampire without being bitten: The narrative collective-assimilation hypothesis. Psychological Science, 221, 990–994.
Gentile, D. A., Anderson, C. A., Yukawa, S., Ihori, N., Saleem, M., Lim, K. M., … Sakamoto, A. (2009). The effects of prosocial video games on prosocial behaviors: International evidence from correlational, longitudinal and experimental studies. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 351, 752–763.
Gerrig, R. (1993). Experiencing narrative worlds. On the psychological activities of reading. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Goldman, A. I. (2006). Simulating minds. The philosophy, psychology and neuroscience of mindreading. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Goldstein, T., & Winner, E. (2012). Enhancing empathy and theory of mind. Cognition and Development, 131, 19–37.
Goldstein, T., Wu, K., and Winner, E. (2009). Actors are skilled in theory of mind but not empathy. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 291, 115–133.
Green, M. C. & Brock, T. C. (2000). The Role of Transportation in the Persuasiveness of Public Narratives. Personality and Social Psychology, 791, 701–721.
Green, M. C. & Brock, T. C. (2002). In the mind’s eye: Transportation-imagery model of narrative persuasion. In M. C. Green, J. J. Strange, & T. C. Brock (Eds.) Narrative impact: social and cognitive foundations (pp. 315–341). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
Hall, A. E. & Bracken, C. C. (2011). “I really liked that movie”: Testing the relationship between trait empathy, transportation, perceived realism and movie enjoyment. Media Psychology, 231, 90–99.
Happé, F. G. E. (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding story characters’ thoughts and feelings by able Autistic, mentally Handicapped, and normal children and adults. Autism and Developmental Disorders, 241, 129–154.
Happé, F. G. E., Winner, E., & Brownell, H. (1998). The getting of wisdom: theory of mind and old age. Developmental Psychology, 341, 358–362.
Heide, F. J., Porter, N., & Saito, P. K. (2012). Do you hear the people sing? Musical theatre and attitude change. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity and the Arts, 61, 224–230.
Hollywood Foreign Press Association. Winners and Nominees Best Motion Picture (n.d.). [URL] Accessed 26.05.17.
Hsu, C., Conrad, M., & Jacobs, A. M. (2014). Fiction Feelings in Harry Potter: haemodynamic responses in the mid-cingulate cortex correlates with immersive reading experience. Neuroport, 251, 1356–1361.
Ickes, W. (Ed.). (1997). Empathic accuracy. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Johnson, D. R. (2012). Transportation into a story increases empathy, prosocial behavior, and perceptual bias toward fearful expressions. Personality and Individual Differences, 521, 150–155.
Keen, S. (2007). Empathy and the novel. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Kidd, D. C., & Castano, E. (2013). Reading literary fiction improves theory of mind. Science, 3421, 377–380.
Kidd, D., & Castano, E. (2017a). Panero et al. (2016): Failure to replicate methods caused failure to replicate results. Personality and Social Psychology, 1121, e–e4.
Kidd, D., & Castano, E. (2017b). Different stories: How levels of familiarity with literary and genre fiction relate to mentalizing. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity and the Arts, 111, 474–486.
Kidd, D., & Castano, E. (2018a). Reading literary fiction and theory of mind: Three preregistered replications and extensions of Kidd and Castano (2013). Social Psychological and Personality Science, 201, 1–10.
Kidd, D., & Castano, E. (2018b). Reading literary fiction can improve theory of mind. Nature Human Behavior, 21, 604,
Klimecki, O. M., Mayer, S. V., Jusyte, A., Scheeff, J., & Schönenberg, M. (2016). Empathy promotes altruistic behavior in economic interactions. Scientific Reports, 6(31961), 1–5.
Konrath, S. (2013). Critical synthesis package: Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI). MedEdPORTAL, 9(9596).
Koopman, E. M. (2015). Empathic reactions after reading. The role of genre, personal factors and affective responses. Poetics, 501, 62–79.
Koopman, E. M. (2016). Effects of “literariness” on emotions and on empathy and reflection after reading. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity and the Arts, 101, 82–98.
Lehne, M., Engel, P., Rohrmeier, M., Menninghaus, M., Jacobs, A. M., & Koelsch, S. (2015). Reading a suspenseful literary text activates brain areas related to social cognition and predictive inference. PLoSOne, 10(5), 1–18.
MacCallum, R. C., Widaman, K. F., Zhang, S., & Hong, S. (1999). Sample size in factor analysis. Psychological Methods, 41, 84–99.
Mar, R. A., & Oatley, K. (2008). The function of fiction is the abstraction and simulation of social experience. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 31, 173–192.
Mar, R. A., Oatley, K., Hirsch, J., dela Paz, J. & Peterson, J. B. (2006). Bookworms versus nerds: Exposure to fiction versus non-fiction, divergent associations with social ability, and the simulation of fictional social worlds. Research in Personality, 401, 694–712.
Mar, R. A., Oatley, K., & Peterson, J. B. (2009). Exploring the link between reading fiction and empathy: Ruling out individual differences and examining outcomes. Communications, 341, 407–428.
Mar, R., Tackett, J. L., & Moore, C. (2010). Exposure to media and theory-of-mind development in preschoolers. Cognitive Development, 251, 69–78.
Maylor, E. A., Moulson, J. M., Muncer, A. M., & Taylor, L. A. (2002). Does performance on theory of mind tasks decline in old age?British Journal of Psychology, 931, 465–485.
McLuhan, M. (1964/1994). Understanding media: The extensions of man. (Rev. ed.) MIT Press: Cambridge, MA.
McQuail, D. (2010). Mass communication theory: An introduction. London, England: Sage.
Miesen, H. (2004). Fiction readers’ appreciation of text attributes in literary and popular novels: Some empirical findings. International Journal of Arts Management, 71, 45–56.
Mol, S. E., & Bus, A. G. (2011). To read or not to read: A meta-analysis of print-exposure from infancy to early adulthood. Psychological Bulletin, 1371, 267–296.
Mumper, M. L., & Gerrig, R. J. (2017). Leisure reading and social cognition: A meta-analysis. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity and the Arts, 111, 109–120.
National Theatre. NT 2000: The Most Significant Plays of the 20th Century (n.d.). [URL] Accessed 09.06.2016
Nell, V. (1988). The psychology of reading for pleasure: Needs and gratifications. Reading Research Quarterly, 231, 6–50.
Nettle, D. (2006). Psychological profiles of professional actors. Personality and Individual Differences, 401, 376–383.
New York Theater. The 50 best plays of past 100 years. (2013). [URL] Accessed 26.05.17.
Nussbaum, M. (2010). Not for profit. Why democracy needs the humanities. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Oatley, K. (1999). Meetings of minds: Dialogue, sympathy, and identification, in reading fiction. Poetics, 261, 439–454.
Oatley, K. (2016). Fiction: Simulation of social worlds. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 201, 618–628.
Paal, T., & Bereczkei, T. (2007). Adult theory of mind, cooperation, Machiavellianism: The effect of mindreading on social relations. Personality and Individual Differences, 431, 541–551.
Panero, M. E., Weisberg, D. S., Black, J., Goldstein, T. R., Barnes, J. L., Brownell, H., & Winner, E. (2016). Does reading a single passage of literary fiction really improve theory of mind? An attempt at replication. Personality and Social Psychology, 1111, 46–54.
Panero, M. E., Weisberg, D. S., Black, J., Goldstein, T. R., Barnes, J. L., Brownell, H., & Winner, E. (2017). No support for the claim that literary fiction uniquely and immediately improves theory of mind: A reply to Kidd and Castano’s commentary on Panero, Weisberg, Black, Goldstein, Barnes, Brownell & Winner (2016). Personality and Social Psychology, 1121, e5–e8.
Penner, L. A., Fritzsche, B. A., Craiger, J. P., & Freifeld, T. R. (1995). Measuring the prosocial personality. In J. Butcher & C. D. Spielberger (Eds.) Advances in personality assessment (Vol. 101.). Hillsdale, NJ: LEA.
Perner, J., & Wimmer, H. (1985). “John thinks that Mary thinks that”: Attribution of second-order beliefs by 5- to 10-year old children. Experimental Child Psychology, 391, 437–471.
Pino, M. C., & Mazza, M. (2016). The use of “literary fiction” to promote mentalizing ability. PloSONE 11(8).
Rushton, J. P., Chrisjohn, R. D., & Fekken, G. C. (1981). The altruistic personality and the self-report altruism scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 11, 292–302.
Samur, D., Tops, M., & Koole, S. L. (2018). Does a single session of reading literary fiction prime enhanced mentalising performance? Four replication experiments of Kidd and Castano (2013). Cognition and Emotion, 321, 130–144. doi
Schellenberg, E. G. (2004). Music lessons enhance IQ. Psychological Science, 151, 511–514.
Seddon, C. (2011). Lifestyles and social participation. Retrieved from Office for National Statistics: [URL].
Singer, T., & Lamm, C. (2009). The social neuroscience of empathy. New York Academy of Sciences, 11561, 91–96.
Speer, N. K., Reynolds, J. R., Swallow, K. M., & Zacks, J. M. (2009). Reading stories activates neural representations of visual and motor experiences. Psychological Science, 201, 989–999.
Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (1989). Exposure to print and orthographic processing. Reading Research Quarterly, 241, 402–433.
Stanovich, K. E., West, R. F., & Harrison, M. R. (1995). Knowledge growth and maintenance across the life span: the role of print exposure. Developmental Psychology, 311, 811–826.
Stephens-Hernandez, A. B., Livingston, J. N., Dacons-Brock, K., Craft, H. L., Cameron, A., Franklin, S. O., & Howlett, A. C. (2007). Drama-based education to motivate participation in substance abuse prevention. Substance Abuse Treatment Prevention Policy, 21, 1–11.
Taylor, M., & Carlson, S. M. (1997). The relationship between individual differences in fantasy and theory of mind. Child Development, 681, 436–455.
Turner, R. & Felisberti, F. M. (2017). Measuring Mindreading: A review of behavioral approaches to measuring “theory of mind” in neurologically typical adults. Frontiers in Psychology, 81, 47.
Valente, T. W., & Bharath, U. (1999). An evaluation of the use of drama to communicate HIV/AIDS information. Education and Prevention, 111, 203–211. Retrieved from [URL]
Valkenburg, P. M., Peter, J., & Walther, J. B. (2016). Media effects: theory and research. Annual Review of Psychology, 671, 315–338.
Wai, M., & Tiliopoulos, N. (2012). The affective and cognitive empathic nature of the dark triad of personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 521, 794–799.
Wallentin, M., Nielsen, A. H., Vuust, P., Dohn, A., Roepstorff, A., & Lund, T. E. (2011). BOLD response to motion verbs in left posterior middle temporal gyrus during story comprehension. Brain & Language, 1191, 221–225.
Winner, E., Brownell, H., Happé, F., Blum, A., & Pincus, D. (1998). Distinguishing lies from jokes: Theory of mind deficits and discourse interpretation in right hemisphere brain-damaged patients. Brain and Language, 621, 89–106.
Zaki, J., & Ochsner, K. (2012). The neuroscience of empathy: progress, pitfalls and promise. Nature Neuroscience, 151, 675–680.
Zillman, D. (1988). Mood management through communication choices. American Behavioral Scientist, 311, 327–340. Retrieved from [URL]
Zunshine, L. (2006). Why we read fiction: theory of mind and the novel. Columbus, OH: Ohio State University Press.
Cited by (8)
Cited by eight other publications
Antonini, Alessio, Sam Brooker & Lovro Škopljanac
2024. Spontaneous transmedia co-location: Integration in memory. Memory, Mind & Media 3
Bortolotti, Alessandro, Alice Conti, Angelo Romagnoli & Pier Luigi Sacco
2024. Imagination vs. routines: festive time, weekly time, and the predictive brain. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 18
Christiansen, Charlotte E.
2023. Does Fiction Reading Make Us Better People? Empathy and Morality in a Literary Empowerment Programme. Ethnos 88:5 ► pp. 994 ff.
Noon, Edward John & Rose Turner
2022. The European Federation of Psychology Students’ Associations Junior Researcher Programme: A Reflection from two Research Project Supervisors. PsyPag Quarterly 1:122 ► pp. 44 ff.
2023. Challenges of measuring empathic accuracy: A mentalizing versus experience‐sharing paradigm. British Journal of Social Psychology 62:2 ► pp. 972 ff.
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 5 november 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.