Article published in:
Scientific Study of Literature
Vol. 9:2 (2019) ► pp. 230239
References

References

Aryani, A., Conrad, M., Schmidtke, D., & Jacobs, A.
(2018) Why ‘piss’ is ruder than ‘pee’? The role of sound in affective meaning making. PloS One, 13(6), e0198430. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Auracher, J.
(2017) Sound iconicity of abstract concepts: Place of articulation is implicitly associated with abstract concepts of size and social dominance. PloS One, 12(11), e0187196. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Auracher, J., Albers, S., Zhai, Y., Gareeva, G., & Stavniychuk, T.
(2011) P Is for Happiness, N Is for Sadness: Universals in Sound Iconicity to Detect Emotions in Poetry. Discourse Processes, 48(1), 1–25. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Becker, J. A., & Fisher, S. K.
(1988) Comparison of associations to vowel speech sounds by English and Spanish speakers. The American Journal of Psychology, 101(1), 51–57. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Börnstein, W.
(1936) On the Functional Relations of the Sense Organs to One Another and to the Organism as a Whole. The Journal of General Psychology, 15(1), 117–131. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
FóNagy, I.
(1961) Communication in Poetry. WORD, 17(2), 194–218. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Greenberg, J. H., & Jenkins, J. J.
(1966) Studies in the Psychological Correlates of the Sound System of American English. WORD, 22(1–3), 207–242. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hornbostel, E. M. V.
(1938 / 1927) The unity of the senses. In W. D. Ellis (Ed.), A source book of Gestalt psychology (pp. 210–216). London: Kegan Paul, Trench, Trubner & Company. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jürgens, U., & Nikolić, D.
(2012) Ideaesthesia: Conceptual processes assign similar colours to similar shapes. Translational Neuroscience, 3(1), 36. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
[ p. 238 ]
Kraxenberger, M., & Menninghaus, W.
(2016) Mimological Reveries? Disconfirming the Hypothesis of Phono-Emotional Iconicity in Poetry. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1779. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lindauer, M. S.
(2013) The Expressiveness of Perceptual Experience: Physiognomy reconsidered. Consciousness & Emotion Book Series. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company. Retrieved from http://​site​.ebrary​.com​/lib​/alltitles​/docDetail​.action​?docID​=10777306. Crossref
Marks, L.
(1978) The unity of the senses: interrelations among the modalities. Academic Press series in cognition and perception. New York [etc.]: Academic Press [etc.].Google Scholar
Martino, G., & Marks, L. E.
(2001) Synesthesia: Strong and weak. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 10(2), 61–65. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Miron, M. S.
(1961) A crosslinguistic investigation of phonetic symbolism. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 62(3), 623–630. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mroczko-Wąsowicz, A., & Nikolić, D.
(2014) Semantic mechanisms may be responsible for developing synesthesia. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 509. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nikolić, D.
(2009) Is synaesthesia actually ideaesthesia? An inquiry into the nature of the phenomenon. Retrieved from http://​www​.danko​-nikolic​.com​/wp​-content​/uploads​/2011​/09​/Synesthesia2009​-Nikolic​-Ideaesthesia​.pdf
Osgood, C. E., Suci, G. J., & Tannenbaum, P. H.
(1957) The measurement of meaning. Oxford, England: University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
Parise, C., & Spence, C.
(2013) Audiovisual cross-modal correspondences in the general population. In J. Simner & E. M. Hubbard (Eds.), Oxford library of psychology. The Oxford handbook of synesthesia (1st ed., pp. 790–815). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Reay, I. E.
(1994) Sound Symbolism. In R. E. Asher (Ed.), The encyclopedia of language and linguistics (1st ed., pp. 4064–4070). Oxford: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
Spence, C.
(2011) Crossmodal correspondences: a tutorial review. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 73(4), 971–995. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Walker, L., & Walker, P.
(2016) Cross-sensory mapping of feature values in the size-brightness correspondence can be more relative than absolute. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 42(1), 138–150. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wescott, R. W.
(1971) Linguistic Iconism. Language, 47(2), 416. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Whissell, C.
(1999) Phonosymbolism and the emotional nature of sounds: evidence of the preferential use of particular phonemes in texts of differing emotional tone. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 89(1), 19–48. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2004) “The sound must seem an echo to the sense”: Pope’s use of sound to convey meaning in his translation of Homer’s Iliad. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 98(3 Pt 1), 859–864. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wiseman, M., & van Peer, W.
(2002) The sound of meaning: An empirical study. In S. Csábi & J. Zerkowitz (Eds.), Textual secrets: The message of the medium (pp. 379–383). Budapest, Hungary: Akadémiai Nyomda Martonvásár.Google Scholar
[ p. 239 ]
Cited by

Cited by other publications

Auracher, Jan, Winfried Menninghaus & Mathias Scharinger
2020. Sound Predicts Meaning: Cross‐Modal Associations Between Formant Frequency and Emotional Tone in Stanzas. Cognitive Science 44:10 Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 13 november 2020. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.