Drawing Kafka’s Castle
An experimental expansion of the theory of cognitive realism
We investigated the effects of narrative perspective on mental imagery by comparing responses to an English translation of Franz Kafka’s Das Schloß (The Castle) in the published version (narrated in the third person) versus an earlier (first-person) draft. We analysed participants’ pencil drawings of their imaginative experience for presence/absence of specific features (K. and the castle) and for image entropy (a proxy for image unpredictability). We also used word embeddings to perform cluster analysis of participants’ verbal free-response testimony, generating thematic clusters independently of experimenter expectations. We found no effects of text version on feature presence or overall entropy, but an effect on entropy variance, which was higher in the third-person condition. There was also an effect of text version on free responses: Readers of the third-person version were more likely to use words associated with mood and atmosphere. We offer conclusions on “Kafkaesque” aesthetics, cognitive realism, and the future of experimental literary studies.
Article outline
- Introduction: Descriptive style, cognitive realism, and narrative perspective
- Hypotheses, methods, measures
- Hypotheses and research questions
- Participants
- Procedure
- Test material and independent variable
- Dependent variables
- Dependent variable 1: Local drawing features
- Dependent variable 2: Global image entropy
- Dependent variable 3: Qualitative drawing analysis
- Dependent variable 4: Word embedding clusters for free text responses
- Results
- Drawing measures: The castle and K
- Image entropy
- Cluster analysis of free verbal responses
- Discussion
- The drawings
- The verbal descriptions of differences between drawing and imagining
- Reader versus text variables, and cognitive realism
- Statement on code
- Acknowledgements
-
References
References (63)
References
Allington, D. (2011). “It actually painted a picture of the village and the sea and the bottom of the sea”: Reading groups, cultural legitimacy, and description in narrative (with reference to John Steinbeck’s The Pearl). Language and Literature,
20
(4), 317–332.
Andersen, S. M., & Schwartz, A. H. (1992). Intolerance of ambiguity and depression: A cognitive vulnerability factor linked to hopelessness. Social Cognition,
10
(3), 271–298.
Bardi, A., Guerra, V. M., & Ramdeny, G. S. D. (2009). Openness and ambiguity intolerance: Their differential relations to well-being in the context of an academic life transition. Personality and Individual Differences,
47
(3), 219–223.
Block, N. (1981). Imagery. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Bortolussi, M., & Dixon, P. (2003). Psychonarratology: Foundations for the empirical study of literary response. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Brown, M. B., & Forsythe, A. B. (1974). Robust tests for the equality of variances. Journal of the American Statistical Association,
69
(346), 364–367.
Bryant, D. J., Tversky, B., & Franklin, N. (1992). Internal and external spatial frameworks for representing described scenes. Journal of memory and language,
31
(1), 74–98.
Budner, S. (1962). Intolerance of ambiguity as a personality variable. Journal of Personality,
30
(1), 29–50.
Bulmer, M. G. (1979). Principles of statistics (2nd ed.). New York: Dover.
Carney, J. (2020). The role of aesthetic style in alleviating anxiety about the future. In J. Carroll, M. Clasen, & E. Jonsson (Eds), Evolutionary perspectives on imaginative culture (pp. 141–159). Cham: Springer.
Clark, A. (2016). Surfing uncertainty: Prediction, action, and the embodied mind. New York: Oxford University Press.
Cohn, D. (1968). K. enters The Castle: On the change of person in Kafka’s manuscript. Euphorion,
62
1, 28–43.
Cohn, D. (1978). Transparent minds: Narrative modes for presenting consciousness in fiction. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Davis, F. C., Neta, M., Kim, M. J., Moran, J. M., & Whalen, P. J. (2016). Interpreting ambiguous social cues in unpredictable contexts. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience,
11
(5), 775–782.
Foglia, L., & O’Regan, J. K. (2016). A new imagery debate: Enactive and sensorimotor accounts. Review of Philosophy and Psychology,
7
(1), 181–196.
Frenkel-Brunswick, E. (1949). Intolerance of ambiguity as emotional and perceptual personality variable. Journal of Personality,
18
(1), 108–143.
Grillon, C., Baas, J. P., Lissek, S., Smith, K., & Milstein, J. (2004). Anxious responses to predictable and unpredictable aversive events. Behavioral Neuroscience,
118
(5), 916–924.
Hakemulder, J., & Koopman, E. (2010). Readers closing in on immoral characters’ consciousness: Effects of free indirect discourse on response to literary narratives. Journal of Literary Theory,
4
(1), 41–62.
Haralick, R. M., Shanmugam, K., & Dinstein, I. H. (1973). Textural features for image classification. IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics,
6
1, 610–621.
Hartung, F., Burke, M., Hagoort, P., & Willems, R. M. (2016). Taking perspective: Personal pronouns affect experiential aspects of literary reading. PLOS ONE,
11
(5), e0154732.
Huth, A. G., De Heer, W. A., Griffiths, T. L., Theunissen, F. E., & Gallant, J. L. (2016). Natural speech reveals the semantic maps that tile human cerebral cortex. Nature,
532
(7600), 453–458.
Ingarden, R. (1965). (3rd ed.) Das literarische Kunstwerk. Mit einem Anhang: Von den Funktionen der Sprache im Theaterschauspiel. Tübingen: Niemeyer.
Jach, H. K., & Smillie, L. D. (2019). To fear or fly to the unknown: Tolerance for ambiguity and Big Five personality traits. Journal of Research in Personality,
79
1, 67–78.
Kafka, F. (1925/1982). Das Schloß. Ed. M. Pasley. New York: Schocken, 1982.
Kaiser, D. H., & Deaver, S. (2009). Assessing attachment with the Bird’s Nest Drawing: A review of the research. Art Therapy,
26
(1), 26–33.
Krippendorff, K. (1986). Information theory: Structural models for qualitative data. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Kuzmičová, A. (2012). Presence in the reading of literary narrative: A case for motor enactment. Semiotica,
189
1, 23–48.
Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, fire, and dangerous things: What categories reveal about the mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Levin, D. T., Momen, N., Drivdahl IV, S. B., & Simons, D. J. (2000). Change blindness blindness: The metacognitive error of overestimating change-detection ability. Visual Cognition,
7
(1–3), 397–412.
Liao, H. I., Yeh, S. L., & Shimojo, S. (2011). Novelty vs. familiarity principles in preference decisions: Task-context of past experience matters. Frontiers in Psychology,
2
1, 431.
Löfstedt, T., Brynolfsson, P., Asklund, T., Nyholm, T., & Garpebring, A. (2019). Gray-level invariant Haralick texture features. PLOS ONE,
14
(2), e0212110.
Marks, D. F. (1973). Visual imagery differences in the recall of pictures. British Journal of Psychology,
64
(1), 17–24.
Millis, K. K. (1995). Encoding discourse perspective during the reading of a literary text. Poetics,
23
(3), 235–253.
Myin, E., & Degenaar, J. (2014). Enactive vision. In L. Shapiro (Ed.), Routledge handbook of embodied cognition (pp. 90–98). Abingdon: Routledge.
Noë, A. (2004). Action in perception. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Noë, A. (2005). Real presence. Philosophical Topics,
33
(1), 235–264.
Pascal, R. (1977). The dual voice: Free indirect speech and its functioning in the nineteenth-century European novel. Manchester: Manchester University Press.
Pearson, J., & Kosslyn, S. M. (2015). The heterogeneity of mental representation: Ending the imagery debate. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,
112
(33), 10089–10092.
Proulx, T., & Heine, S. J. (2009). Connections from Kafka: Exposure to meaning threats improves implicit learning of an artificial grammar. Psychological Science,
20
(9), 1125–1131.
Pylyshyn, Z. W. (2003). Seeing and visualizing: It’s not what you think. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Reddy, L., Tsuchiya, N., & Serre, T. (2010). Reading the mind’s eye: Decoding category information during mental imagery. Neuroimage,
50
(2), 818–825.
Rosch, E. (1975). Cognitive representations of semantic categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,
104
(3), 192–233.
Roth, N., Lev-Wiesel, R., & Shochat, T. (2019). “How do you sleep?” Sleep in self-figure drawings of young adolescents in residential care facilities – An exploratory study. Sleep Medicine,
60
1, 116–122.
Salem, S., Weskott, T., & Holler, A. (2017). Does narrative perspective influence readers’ perspective-taking? An empirical study on free indirect discourse, psycho-narration and first-person narration. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics,
2
(1), 61.
Sato, M., Sakai, H., Wu, J., & Bergen, B. (2012). Towards a cognitive science of literary style: Perspective-taking in processing omniscient versus objective voice. In Proceedings of the 34th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 959–964.
Seriès, P., & Seitz, A. (2013). Learning what to expect (in visual perception). Frontiers in Human Neuroscience,
7
1, 668.
Shannon, C. E. (1951). Prediction and entropy of printed English. In Bell System Technical Journal,
30
(1), 50–64.
Simons, D. J., & Rensink, R. A. (2005). Change blindness: Past, present, and future. Trends in cognitive sciences,
9
(1), 16–20.
Swami, V., Stieger, S., Pietschnig, J., & Voracek, M. (2010). The disinterested play of thought: Individual differences and preference for surrealist motion pictures. Personality and Individual Differences,
48
(7), 855–859.
Thomas, N. J. T. (2014). Mental imagery. In E. N. Zalta (Ed.), The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy (Fall 2020 Ed.). [URL]
Troscianko, E. T. (2010). Kafkaesque worlds in real time. Language and Literature,
19
(2), 151–171.
Troscianko, E.T. (2012). The cognitive realism of memory in Flaubert’s Madame Bovary
. Modern Language Review, 1071, 772–795.
Troscianko, E. T. (2013). Reading imaginatively: The imagination in cognitive science and cognitive literary studies. Journal of Literary Semantics,
42
(2), 181–198.
Troscianko, E. T. (2014a). Kafka’s cognitive realism. Abingdon: Routledge.
Troscianko, E. T. (2014b). Reading Kafka enactively. Paragraph,
37
(1), 15–31.
Van Lissa, C. J., Caracciolo, M., van Duuren, T., & van Leuveren, B. (2016). Difficult empathy: The effect of narrative perspective on readers’ engagement with a first-person narrator. Diegesis,
5
(1), 43–63.
van Peer, W., & H. Pander Maat. (1996). Perspectivation and sympathy: Effects of narrative point of view. In R. J. Kruez & M. S. MacNeally (Eds), Empirical approaches to literature and aesthetics (pp. 143–156). New York: Ablex.
van Peer, W., & Pander Maat, H. (2001). Narrative perspective and the interpretation of characters’ motives. Language and Literature,
10
1, 229–241.
Walton, Kendall L. (1990). Mimesis as make-believe: On the foundations of the representational arts. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Wilson, D., & Sperber, D. (2004). Relevance theory. In L. R. Horn & G. L. Ward (Eds), The handbook of pragmatics (pp. 607–632). Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Wittchen, H. U., & Hoyer, J. (2001). Generalized anxiety disorder: Nature and course. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry,
62
(11), 15–21.
Cited by (1)
Cited by one other publication
Riestra-Camacho, Rocío, James Carney & Emily Troscianko
2024.
Can Narrative Bibliotherapy Reduce Vulnerability to Eating Disorders? Evidence from a Reading Experiment.
Empirical Studies of the Arts 42:2
► pp. 303 ff.
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 27 september 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.