Chapter published in:
Tone Orthography and Literacy: The voice of evidence in ten Niger-Congo languages
Edited by David Roberts and Stephen L. Walter
[Studies in Written Language and Literacy 18] 2021
► pp. 235249
References

References

Allington, Richard L.
(1983) Fluency: the neglected reading goal. The Reading Teacher, 36, 556–561.Google Scholar
Anderson, Alida, & Wang, Min
(2012) The utility of Chinese tone processing skill in detecting children with English reading disabilities. Journal of Research in Reading, 35(3), 227–250.Google Scholar
Badejo, B. Rotimi Maiduguri
(1989) An experimental study of tone marking in Bura. Frankfurter Afrikanistische Blätter, 1, 44–51.Google Scholar
Bernard, Russell H., Mbeh, George N., & Handwerker, W. Penn
(1995) The tone problem. In A. Traill, R. Vossen & M. Biesele (Eds.), The complete linguist: Papers in memory of Patrick J. Dickens (pp. 27–44). Cologne: Rüdiger Köppe.Google Scholar
(2002) Does marking tone make tone languages easier to read? Human organisation, 61(4), 339–349.Google Scholar
Bird, Steven
(1999) When marking tone reduces fluency: An orthography experiment in Cameroon. Language and Speech, 42, 83–115.Google Scholar
Burnham, D., Kim, J., Davis, C., Ciocca, V., Schoknecht, C., & Kasisopa, B.
(2011) Are tones phones? Journal of Experimental child psychology, 108, 693–712.Google Scholar
Chen, Jenn-Yeu, Lin, Wei-Chun, & Ferrand, Ludovic
(2003) Masked priming of the syllable in Mandarin Chinese speech production. Chinese Journal of Psychology, 45, 107–120.Google Scholar
Chirkova, Katia, & Michaud, Alexis
(2009) Approaching the prosodic system of Shixing. Language and Linguistics, 10(3), 539–568.Google Scholar
Chuoying Ouyang, Iris, & Kaiser, Elsi
(2015) Prosody and information structure in a tone language: an investigation of Mandarin Chinese. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 30(1–2) (Special Issue: Prosody in Context)), 57–72.Google Scholar
Davis, Chris, Schoknecht, Colin, Kim, Jeesun, & Burnham, Denis
(2016) The Time course for processing vowels and lexical tones: Reading aloud Thai words. Language and Speech, 59(2), 196–218.Google Scholar
Dowhower, S. L.
(1987) Effects of repeated reading on second-grade transitional readings’ fluency and comprehension. Reading research quarterly, 22, 389–406.Google Scholar
(1991) Speaking of prosody: Fluency’s unattended bedfellow. Theory into Practice, 30(3), 165–175.Google Scholar
Duitsman, John
(1986) Testing two systems for marking tone in Western Krahn. Notes on Literacy, 49, 2–10.Google Scholar
Essien, Udo E.
(1977) To end ambiguity in a tone language. In P. F. A. Kotey & H. Der-Houssikian (Eds.), Languages and linguistics problems in Africa. Proceedings of the 7th conference on African linguistics (pp. 155–167). Columbia, South Carolina: Hornbeam Press.Google Scholar
Hudson, Roxanne F., Lane, Holly B., & Pullen, Paige C.
(2005) Reading fluency assessment and instruction: What, why, and how? The Reading Teacher, 58(8), 702–714.Google Scholar
Huff, Christopher, & Payne, Elinor
(2012) Disambiguation of tonally unspecified Mandarin syllables. In Q. Ma (Ed.), Proceedings of the Sixth International Conference on Speech Prosody 2012 (pp. 107–110). Shanghai: Tongji University Press.Google Scholar
Klem, Herbert V.
(1982) Special difficulties in reading tone languages. In H. V. Klem (Ed.), Oral communication of the Scriptures: Insights from African oral art (pp. 19–25). Pasadena, CA: William Carey Library.Google Scholar
Kuhn, Melanie R., Schwanenflugel, Paula, & Meisinger, Elizabeth B.
(2010) Aligning theory and assessment of reading fluency: Automaticity, prosody, and definitions of fluency. Reading research quarterly, 45(2), 230–251.Google Scholar
Lee, C.-Y.
(2007) Does horse activate mother? Processing lexical tone in form priming. Language and Speech, 50(1), 101–123.Google Scholar
Li, J., Xu, B., & Mok, P.
(2014) Effect of orthography on L2 production of Mandarin tones. In C. Gussenhoven, Y. Chen & D. Dediu (Eds.), Proceedings of the 4th International Symposium on Tonal Aspects of Languages (TAL 2014), 13–16 May 2014, Nijmegen (pp. 147–151). Baixas: International Speech Communication Association (ISCA) Archive.Google Scholar
McBride-Chang, Catherine, Tong, Xiuli, Shu, Hua, Wong, Anita M.-Y., Leung, Ka-wai, & Tardif, Twila
(2008) Syllable, phoneme, and tone: Psycholinguistic units in early chinese and English word recognition. Scientific Studies of Reading, 12(2), 171–194.Google Scholar
Mfonyam, Joseph Ngwa
(1989) Tone in orthography: The case of Bafut and related languages. (Thèse d’état), University of Yaounde, Yaoundé.Google Scholar
Pinnell, G. S., Pikulski, J. J., Wixson, K. K., Campbell, J. R., Gough, P. B., & Beatty, A. S.
(1995) Listening to children read aloud: Data from NAEP’s integrated reading performance record (IRPR) at grade 4. Washington DC: Office of Educational Research and Improvement, US Department of Education.Google Scholar
Piper, Benjamin, & Zuilkowski, Stephanie Simmons
(2015) The role of timing in assessing oral reading fluency and comprehension in Kenya. Language testing, 33(1), 75–98.Google Scholar
Rialland, Annie
(2007) Question prosody: An African perspective. In C. Gussenhoven & R. T. (Eds.), Tones and tunes: Studies in word and sentence prosody (Vol. 1, pp. 35–62). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Roberts, David, Basnight-Brown, Dana, & Vydrin, Valentin
(2019) Marking tone with punctuation: An orthography experiment in Eastern Dan. In Y. Haralambous (Ed.), Proceedings of the conference “Grafematik: Graphemics in the 21st century from graphemes to knowledge” (pp. 315–349). Brest, France: Fluxus.Google Scholar
Roberts, David, & Walter, Stephen L.
(2012) Writing grammar rather than tone: An orthography experiment in Togo. Written Language & Literacy, 15(2), 226–253.Google Scholar
(2016) Writing morphophonology, reading lexical tone: Quantitative evidence in favor of morphographic spelling in Kabiye (Togo). Writing Systems Research, 8(2), 167–186.Google Scholar
Schreiber, P. A.
(1980) On the acquisition of reading fluency. Journal of Reading Behavior, 7(3), 177–186.Google Scholar
(1991) Understanding prosody’s role in reading acquisition. Theory into Practice, 30(3), 158–164.Google Scholar
Shu, Hua, Peng, Hong, & McBride-Chang, Catherine
(2008) Phonological awareness in young Chinese children. Developmental Science, 11(1), 171–181.Google Scholar
Siok, W. T., & Fletcher, P.
(2001) The role of phonological awareness and visual-orthographic skills in Chinese reading acquisition. Development Psychology, 37, 886–899.Google Scholar
Tong, Xiuli, He, Xinjie, & Deacon, S. Hélène
(2017) Tone matters for Cantonese-English bilingual children’s English word reading development: A unified model of phonological transfer. Memory & Cognition, 45(2), 320–333.Google Scholar
Tong, Xiuli, Tong, Xiu hong, & McBride-Chang, Catherine
(2015) A tale of two writing systems: Double dissociation and metalinguistic transfer between Chinese and English word reading among Hong Kong children. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 48(2), 130–145.Google Scholar
Torgesen, Joseph K., & Hudson, Roxanne F.
(2006) Reading fluency: Critical issues for struggling readers. In S. J. Samuels & A. E. Farstrup (Eds.), What research has to say about fluency instruction (pp. 130–158). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.Google Scholar
Wang, M., Anderson, A., C. Cheng, Park, Y., & Thomson, J.
(2008) General auditory processing, Chinese tone processing, English phonemic processing, and English reading skill: A comparison between Chinese-English and Korean-English bilingual children. Reading & Writing: an Interdisciplinary Journal, 21(6), 627–644.Google Scholar
Wang, M., Li, C., & Lin, C. Y.
(2015) The contributions of segmental and suprasegmental information in reading Chinese charactes aloud. PLoS ONE, 10(11), e01452060.Google Scholar
Wang, M., Perfetti, C. A., & Liu, Y.
(2005) Chinese-English biliteracy acquisition: Cross-language and writing systems transfer. Cognition, 97, 67–88.Google Scholar
Wang, M., Yang, C., & Cheng, C.
(2009) The contributions of phonology, orthography, and morphology in Chinese-English biliteracy acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 30, 291–314.Google Scholar
Wang, Min, & Gao, Wei
(2011) Subsyllabic unit preference in learning to read pinyin syllables. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 36(2), 142–151.Google Scholar
Wiener, Seth, & Ito, Kiwako
(2015) Do syllable-specific tonal probabilities guide lexical access? Evidence from Mandarin, Shanghai and Cantonese speakers. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 30(9), 1048–1060.Google Scholar
Winskel, Heather
(2011) Orthographic and phonological parafoveal processing of consonants, vowels, and tones when reading Thai. Applied Psycholinguistics, 32, 739–759.Google Scholar
Winskel, Heather, & Iemwanthong, Kanyarat
(2010) Reading and spelling acquisition in Thai children. Reading and Writing, 23(9), 1021–1053.Google Scholar
Winskel, Heather, & Perea, Manuel
(2014) Does tonal information affect the early stages of visual-word processing in Thai? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(2), 209–219.Google Scholar
Winskel, Heather, Ratitamkul, Theeraporn, & Charoensit, Akira
(2017) The role of tone and segmental information in visual-word recognition in Thai. The quarterly journal of experimental psychology, 70(7), 1282–1291.Google Scholar
Xu, B., Li, J., & Mok, P.
(2014) The effect of orthography on L2 perception. In C. Gussenhoven, Y. Chen & D. Dediu (Eds.), Proceedings of the 4th International Symposium on Tonal Aspects of Languages (TAL 2014) 13–16 May, 2014, Nijmegen (pp. 75–78). Baixas: International Speech Communication Association (ISCA) Archive.Google Scholar