Article In:
Translation and Interpreting at the Interface of Cognition and Emotion
Edited by Ana María Rojo López and Purificación Meseguer Cutillas
[Translation, Cognition & Behavior 7:1] 2024
► pp. 100118
References (68)
Bauman, C. W., McGraw, A. P., Bartels, D. M.; Warren, C.
(2014) Revisiting External Validity: Concerns about Trolley Problems and Other Sacrificial Dilemmas in Moral Psychology. Social and Personality Psychology Compass. 8 ( 9 ), 536–554. DOI logo. S2CID 11170070.Google Scholar
Brander de la Iglesia, M.
(2017) ‘A Sea of Troubles’: ethical dilemmas from war zones to the classroom. In C. Valero-Garcés, & R. Tipton (Eds.), Ideology, Ethics and Policy Development in Public Service Interpreting and Translation (Translation, Interpreting and Social Justice in a Globalised World 1) (pp. 84–101). Multilingual Matters. [URL]. DOI logo
Brouwer, S.
(2019) The auditory foreign-language effect of moral decision making in highly proficient bilinguals. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development 40 1, 865–878. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2020) The interplay between emotion and modality in the foreign language effect on moral decision making. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1–8.Google Scholar
Cipolletti, H., S. McFarlane, & C. Weissglass
(2016) The moral foreign language effect. Philosophical Psychology 29 1, 23–40. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cokely, D.
(2000) Exploring ethics: A case for revising the Code of Ethics. Journal of Interpretation, 25 –60Google Scholar
Costa, A., A. Foucart, S. Hayakawa, M. Aparici, J. Apesteguia, J. Heafner. & B. Keysar
(2014) Your morals depend on language. PloS one 9 1, e94842. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crockett, M. J.
(2016) How formal models can illuminate mechanisms of moral judgment and decision making. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25 ( 2 ), 85–90. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cukur, L.
(2024) Towards an ethical framework for evaluating paid translation crowdsourcing and its consequences. In Zwischenberger, Cornelia and Leandra Cukur, (Eds.), Translation on and over the web: disentangling conceptual uncertainties and ethical questions. Special issue of The Translator. Studies in Intercultural Communication 30 ( 1 ): 1–121: 47–62. DOI logo. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cushman, F. A.
(2015) From moral concern to moral constraint. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 3 1, 58–62. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cushman, F. A., Young, L. L., & Hauser, M. D.
(2006) The role of conscious reasoning and intuition in moral judgment: Testing three principles of harm. Psychological Science, 17 ( 12 ), 1082–1089. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dean, R., & R. Q. Jr Pollard
(2011) Context-based ethical reasoning in interpreting: a demand control schema perspective. In M. Baker & C. Maier (Eds.), Ethics and the curriculum: critical perspectives. Special issue of The Interpreter and Translator Trainer 5 ( 1 ), 155–182. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dean, R.
(2015) Sign language interpreters’ ethical discourse and moral reasoning patterns. PhD Dissertation, Heriot-Watt University.
(2021) Healthcare interpreting ethics. In S. Şebnem & E. Spišiaková (Eds.), The Routledge Handbook of Translation and Health (pp. 198–215). Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Del Maschio, N., Crespi, F., Peressotti, F., Abutalebi, J., Sulpizio, S.
(2022) Decision-making depends on language: A meta-analysis of the Foreign Language Effect. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 25 1, 617–630. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Drugan, J.
(2020) Complex collaborations: interpreting and translating for the UK police. In A. Alfer, A. & C. Zwischenberger (Eds.), Translaboration: exploring collaboration in translation and translation in collaboration. Special issue of Target. International Journal on Translation Studies 32 ( 2 ), 307–326. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Geipel, J., C. Hadjichristidis. & L. Surian
(2015a) How foreign language shapes moral judgment. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 59 1, 8–17. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015b) The foreign language effect on moral judgment: The role of emotions and norms. PloS one 10 1, e0131529.r. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Geipel, J., Hadjichristidis, C., & Surian, L.
(2016) Foreign language affects the contribution of intentions and outcomes to moral judgment. Cognition 154 1, 34–39. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gonzalez-Davies, M., & Enríquez Raído, V.
(Eds.) (2018) Situated learning in translator and interpreter training: Bridging research and good practice. Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Greene, J. D.
(2003) From neural “is” to moral “ought”: What are the moral implications of neuroscientific moral psychology? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4 1, 847–850. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2013) Moral tribes. Emotions, reason, and the gap between us and them. The Penguin Press.Google Scholar
(2014) Beyond point-and-shoot morality. Ethics, 124 ( 4 ), 695–726. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Greene, J., Sommerville, R. B., Nystrom, L. E., Darley, J. M., & Cohen, J. D.
(2001) An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science, 293 1, 2105–2108. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Greene, J. D., Morelli, S. A., Lowenberg, K., Nystrom, L. E., & Cohen, J. D.
(2008) Cognitive load selectively interferes with utilitarian moral judgment. Cognition 107 1, 1144–1154. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Greene, J. D., Nystrom, L. E., Engell, A. D., Darley, J. M., & Cohen, J. D.
(2004) The neural bases of cognitive conflict and control in moral judgment. Neuron, 44 ( 2 ), 389–400. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Guerberof-Arenas, A. & Joss Moorkens
(2023) Ethics and Machine Translation: the end user perspective. In Moniz, Helena and Carla Parra Escartín, (Eds.), Towards Responsible Machine Translation: ethical and legal considerations in machine translation. Cham: Springer. pp. 113–133. DOI logo. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Haidt, J.
(2001) The emotional dog and its rational tail. Psychological Review, 108 ( 4 ), 814–834. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012) The righteous mind: Why good people are divided by politics and religion. New York: Pantheon.Google Scholar
Haidt, J. & F. Bjorklund
(2008) Social intuitionists answer six questions about moral psychology. In W. Sinnott-Armstrong in Moral Psychology (Ed.), The Cognitive Science of Morality: Intuition and Diversity (pp. 181–217). Volume 21. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Haidt, J., & Kesebir, S.
(2010) Morality. In S. Fiske, D. Gilbert, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology, 5th ed. (pp. 797–832). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hauser, M.
(2006) Moral minds: How nature designed our universal sense of right and wrong. New York: Ecco/Harper Collins.Google Scholar
Hauser, M., Young, L., & Cushman, F.
(2008) Reviving Rawls’ linguistic analogy: Operative principles and the causal structure of moral actions. In W. Sinnott-Armstrong (Ed.), Moral psychology: Vol. 2. The cognitive science of morality (pp. 107–143). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hayakawa, S., Tannenbaum, D., Costa, A., Corey, J. D., & Keysar, B.
(2017) Thinking more or feeling less? Explaining the foreign-language effect on moral judgment. Psychological science 28 1, 1387–1397. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Horváth, I.
(2022) AI in interpreting: ethical considerations. Across Languages and Cultures 23 ( 1 ), 1–13. [URL]. DOI logo
Hu, Bei
(2024) Negotiation, power and ethics in online collaborative translation: translation of “COVID-19” by Wikipedia translator-editors. In Zwischenberger, Cornelia and Leandra Cukur, eds. Translation on and over the web: disentangling conceptual uncertainties and ethical questions. Special issue of The Translator. Studies in Intercultural Communication 30(1): 1–12: 78–95. DOI logo. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kahane, G., & Shackel, N.
(2008) Do abnormal responses show utilitarian bias? Nature, 452 ( 7185 ), E5. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kahane, G., Everett, J. A., Earp, B. D., Caviola, L., Faber, N. S., Crockett, M. J., & Savulescu, J.
(2018) Beyond sacrificial harm: A two-dimensional model of utilitarian psychology. Psychological review, 125 ( 2 ), 131. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D.
(2011) Thinking, fast and slow. New York: Farrar, Straus, and Giroux.Google Scholar
Keysar, B., S. L. Hayakawa, & S. G. An
(2012) The foreign-language effect: Thinking in a foreign tongue reduces decision biases. Psychological science 23 1, 661–668. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kohlberg, L.
(1969) Stage and sequence: The cognitive-developmental spproach to socialization. In D. Goslin (Ed.), Handbook of Socialization Theory and Research (pp. 347–480). Rand McNally and Company.Google Scholar
(1984) The psychology of moral development: The nature and validity of moral stages. San Francisco: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Korpal, P., & Mellinger, C. D.
(2022) Self-care strategies of professional community interpreters: An interview-based study. Translation, Cognition & Behavior, 5 ( 2 ), 275–299. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Koskinen, K., & Pokorn, N. K.
(Eds.) (2021) The Routledge handbook of translation and ethics. London and New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Krajbich, I., Bartling, B., Hare, T., & Fehr, E.
(2015) Rethinking fast and slow based on a critique of reaction-time reverse inference. Nature Communications, 6 ( 1 ), 7455. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leneham, M., & J. Napier
(2003) Sign language interpreters’ codes of ethics: Should we maintain the status quo? Deaf Worlds, 19 1, 78–98.Google Scholar
Levy, N.
(2014) Neuroethics: moral cognition. In Oxford handbooks online. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Marian, V., & Neisser, U.
(2000) Language-dependent recall of autobiographical memories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 129 1, 361. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marín García, A.
(2024/in press). Interpreting and moral cognition. In C. Mellinger (Ed.), Routledge Handbook on Interpreting Studies. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
May, J.
(2018) Regard for reason in the moral mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
May, J., Workman, C. I., Haas, J., & Han, H.
(2022) The neuroscience of moral judgment: empirical and philosophical developments. In F. De Brigard & W. Sinnott-Armstrong (Eds.), Neuroscience and Philosophy (pp-). MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McGuire, J., Langdon, R., Coltheart, M., & Mackenzie, C.
(2009) A reanalysis of the personal/impersonal distinction in moral psychology research. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45 ( 3 ), 577–580. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mikkelson, H.
Miller, C. B.
(2016) Assessing two competing approaches to the psychology of moral judgments. Philosophical Explorations 19 1, 28–47. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2021) Moral Psychology. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Monzó-Nebot, E., & Wallace, M.
(2020) New societies, new values, new demands: Mapping non-professional interpreting and translation, remapping translation and interpreting ethics. Translation and Interpreting Studies. The Journal of the American Translation and Interpreting Studies Association, 15 ( 1 ), 1–14. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nelkin, D. K., McKenzie, C. R., Rickless, S. C., & Ryazanov, A. A.
(2023) Trolley problems reimagined: Sensitivity to ratio, risk, and comparisons. In H. Viciana, A. Gaitán, & G. Aguiar (Eds.), Experiments in moral and political philosophy (pp. 62–82). Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Railton, P.
(2014) The affective dog and its rational tail: Intuition and attunement. Ethics, 124 1, 813–859. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017) Moral learning: Conceptual foundations and normative relevance. Cognition, 167 1, 172–190. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rest, J. R.
(1984) Research on moral development: Implications for training counseling psychologists. The Counseling Psychologist, 12 1, 19–29. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ruiz Rosendo, L.
Schrauf, R. W., & Rubin, D. C.
(2000) Internal languages of retrieval: The bilingual encoding of memories for the personal past. Memory & Cognition 28 1, 616–623. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Thomson, J. J.
(1976) Killing, letting die, and the trolley problem. The Monist, 59 1, 204–217. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Viciana, H., Gaitán, A., & González, F. A.
(Eds.) (2024) Experiments in Moral and Political Philosophy. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Vives, M. L., Aparici, M., & Costa, A.
(2018) The limits of the foreign language effect on decision-making: The case of the outcome bias and the representativeness heuristic. PloS one 13 1, e0203528. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tversky, A. & D. Kahneman
(1981) The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science 211 1, 453–458. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zhou, M.
(2022) Educating translation ethics: a neurocognitive ethical decision-making approach. The Interpreter and Translator Trainer, 1–18. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zimányi, K.
(2009) A diagrammatic approach to redefining the role of the interpreter based on a case study in forensic psychology. Translation & Interpreting 1 ( 2 ), 55–70.Google Scholar