References
Akhtar, N., & Gernsbacher, M.A
(2008) On privileging the role of gaze in infant social cognition. Child Development Perspectives , 2(2), 59–65. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Allwood, J
(1981) On the distinction between semantics and pragmatics. In W. Klein & W. ­Levelt (Eds.), Crossing the Boundaries in Linguistics (pp. 174–189). Dordrecht: Reidel. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ariel, M
(2010)  Defining Pragmatics : Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Austin, J.L
(1962)  How to Do Things with Words . Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
Bakeman, R., & Adamson, L.B
(1984) Coordinating attention to people and objects in mother-infant and peer-infant interaction. Child Development , 55(4), 1278–1289. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baldwin, D.A
(1991) Infants’ contribution to the achievement of joint reference. Child Development , 62(5), 875–890. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bannard, C., & Tomasello, M
(2012) Can we dissociate contingency learning from social learning in word acquisition by 24-month-olds? PLoS ONE , 7(11), e49881. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S
(1989) Perceptual role taking and protodeclarative pointing in autism. British Journal of Developmental Psychology , 7(2), 113–127. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bates, E., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V
(1975) The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly of Behavior and Development , 21(3), 205–226.Google Scholar
Bates, E
(1976)  Language and Context: Studies in the Acquisition of Pragmatics . New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
(2003) Natura e cultura nel linguaggio [On the nature and nurture of language]. In E. Bizzi, P. Calissano & V. Volterra (Eds.), Frontiere della Biologia: Il Cervello di Homo Sapiens [Frontiers of Biology: The Brain of Homo Sapiens] (pp. 241–265). Rome: Istituto della Enciclopedia Italiana fondata da Giovanni Trecanni.Google Scholar
Bateson, M.C
(1979) The epigenesis of conversational interaction: A personal account of research and development. In M. Bullowa (Ed.), Before Speech: The Beginnings of Human Communication (pp. 63–77). London: CUP.Google Scholar
Batki, A., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Connellan, J., & Ahluwalia, J
(2000) Is there an innate gaze module? Evidence from human neonates. Infant Behavior and Development , 23(2), 223–229. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Beaumont, S.L., & Bloom, K
(1993) Adults’ attributions of intentionality to vocalizing infants. First Language , 13(38), 235–247. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Behne, T., Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M
(2005) Unwilling versus unable: Infants’ understanding of intentional action. Developmental Psychology , 41(2), 328–337. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bergelson, E., & Swingley, D
(2012) At 6–9 months, human infants know the meanings of many common nouns. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences , 109(9), 3253–3258. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Breheny, R
(2006) Communication and folk psychology. Mind & Language , 21(1), 74–107. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brooks, R., & Meltzoff, A.N
(2005) The development of gaze following and its relation to language. Developmental Science , 8(6), 535–543. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J
(1981) The social context of language acquisition. Language & Communication , 1(2–3), 155–178. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J.S
(1983)  Child’s Talk . New York, NY: Norton.Google Scholar
Bushneil, I., Sai, F., & Mullin, J
(1989) Neonatal recognition of the mother’s face. British Journal of Developmental Psychology , 7(1), 3–15. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Butterworth, G.E
(2003) Pointing is the royal road to language for babies. In S. Kita (Ed.), Pointing: Where Language, Culture, and Cognition Meet (pp. 9–33). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Butterworth, G.E., & Grover, L
(1988) The origins of referential communication in human infancy. In L. Weiskrantz (Ed.), Thought without Language (pp. 5–25). Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
Camaioni, L
(1993) The development of intentional communication: A re-analysis. In J. Nadel & L. Camaioni (Eds.), New Perspectives in Early Communicative Development (pp. 82–96). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Callaghan, T., Moll, H., Rakoczy, H., Behne, T., Liszkowski, U., & Tomasello, M
(2011) Early social cognition in three cultural contexts. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development , 76(2), 1–142.Google Scholar
Cameron-Faulkner, T
(2013) The interaction of gesture, intonation, and eye-gaze in proto-imperatives. Journal of Child Language, FirstView , 1–19. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carpendale, J.I.M., & Carpendale, A.B
(2010) The development of pointing: From personal directedness to interpersonal direction. Human Development , 53(3), 110–126. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carpendale, J.I.M., & Lewis, C
(2004) Constructing an understanding of mind: The development of children’s social understanding within social interaction. Behavioral and Brain Sciences , 27(1), 79–96. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, M
(2012) Joint attention in humans and animals. In N. Seel (Ed.), Encyclopedia of the Sciences of Learning (pp. 1663–1664). Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
Carpenter, M., Akhtar, N., & Tomasello, M
(1998) Fourteen- through 18-month-old infants differentially imitate intentional and accidental actions. Infant Behavior and Development , 21(2), 315–330. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., & Tomasello, M
(1998) Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development , 63(4), i-iv, 1–143. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, R.L.T.U.o.W., Mastergeorge, A.M.P.S., Hearing, C., & Coggins, T.E.T.U.o.W
(1983) The acquisition of communicative intentions in infants eight to fifteen months of age. Language and Speech , 26, 101.Google Scholar
Clark, H.H., & Wilkes-Gibbs, D
(1986) Referring as a collaborative process. Cognition , 22(1), 1–39. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Colonnesi, C., Stams, G.J.J.M., Koster, I., & Noom, M.J
(2010) The relation between pointing and language development: A meta-analysis. Developmental Review , 30(4), 352–366. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cooper, R.P., & Aslin, R.N
(1990) Preference for infant-directed speech in the first month after birth. Child Development , 61, 1584–1595. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Corkum, V., & Moore, C
(1995) Development of joint visual attention in infants. In C. Moore & P.J. Dunham (Eds.), Joint Attention: Its Origins and Role in Development (pp. 61–83). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Csibra, G
(2010) Recognizing communicative intentions in infancy. Mind & Language , 25(2), 141–168. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Csibra, G., & Gergely, G
(2009) Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences , 13(4), 148–153. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Daum, M.M., Ulber, J., & Gredeback, G
(2013) The development of pointing perception in infancy: Effects of communicative signals on covert shifts of attention. Developmental Psychology , 49(10), 1898–1908. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
D’Entremont, B., Hains, S.M.J., & Muir, D.W
(1997) A demonstration of gaze following in 3- to 6-month-olds. Infant Behavior and Development , 20(4), 569–572. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Daum, M.M., Ulber, J., & Gredeback, G
(2013) The development of pointing perception in infancy: Effects of communicative signals on covert shifts of attention. Developmental Psychology , 49(10), 1898–1908. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
DeCasper, A.J., & Fifer, W.P
(1980) Of human bonding: Newborns prefer their mothers’ voices. Science , 208(4448), 1174–1176. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Eilan, N
(2005) Joint attention, communication and mind. In N. Eilan, C. Hoerl, T. ­McCormack & J. Roessler (Eds.), Joint Attention: Communication and Other Minds (pp. 1–33). Oxford: OUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Esteve-Gibert, N., & Prieto, P
(2012) Prosody signals the emergence of intentional communication in the first year of life: Evidence from Catalan-babbling infants. Journal of Child Language, FirstView , 1–26.Google Scholar
Farroni, T., Csibra, G., Simion, F., & Johnson, M.H
(2002) Eye contact detection in humans from birth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences , 99(14), 9602–9605. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Farroni, T., Menon, E., & Johnson, M.H
(2006) Factors influencing newborns’ preference for faces with eye contact. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology , 95(4), 298–308. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fernald, A
(1993) Approval and disapproval: Infant responsiveness to vocal affect in familiar and unfamiliar languages. Child Development , 64(3), 657–674. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fernald, A., Taeschner, T., Dunn, J., Papousek, M., de Boysson-Bardies, B., & Fukui, I
(1989) A cross-language study of prosodic modifications in mothers’ and fathers’ speech to preverbal infants. Journal of Child Language , 16(03), 477–501. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Field, T., Healy, B., Goldstein, S., Perry, S., & Bendell, D
(1988) Infants of depressed mothers show “depressed” behavior even with nondepressed adults. Child Development , 59(6), 1569–1579. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fillmore, C.J
(1971) Verbs of judging: An exercise in semantic description. In C.J. Fillmore & D.T. Langendoen, (Eds.), Studies in Linguistic Semantics (pp. 273–289). New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Gergely, G., & Watson, J
(1999) Early socio-emotional development: Contingency perception and the social biofeedback model. In P. Rochat (Ed.), Early Social Cognition: Understanding Others in the First Months of Life (pp. 101–136). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Gergely, G., Bekkering, H., & Király, I
(2002) Developmental psychology: Rational imitation in preverbal infants. Nature , 415(6873), 755. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Golinkoff, R.M
(1983) The preverbal negotiation of failed messages: Insights into the transition period. In R.M. Golinkoff (Ed.), The Transition from Prelinguistic to Linguistic Communication (pp. 57–78). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
(1993) When is communication a ‘meeting of minds’? Journal of Child Language , 20(1), 199–207. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Golinkoff, R.M., & Gordon, L
(1988) What makes communication run? Characteristics of immediate successes. First Language , 8(23), 103–124. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goren, C.C., Sarty, M., & Wu, P.Y.K
(1975) Visual following and pattern discrimination of face-like stimuli by newborn infants. Pediatrics , 56, 544–549.Google Scholar
Grice, H.P
(1957) Meaning. The Philosophical Review , 66(3), 377–388. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grice. H.P
(1975) Logic and conversation. In P. Cole & J.L. Morgan (Eds.), Syntax and Semantics, Vol 3, Speech acts . New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Grosse, G., Behne, T., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M
(2010) Infants communicate in order to be understood. Developmental Psychology , 46(6), 1710–1722. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grosse, G., Scott-Phillips, T.C., & Tomasello, M
(2013, March11). Three-year-olds hide their communicative intentions in appropriate contexts. Developmental Psychology . Advance online publication.
Grossmann, T., Johnson, M.H., Lloyd-Fox, S., Blasi, A., Deligianni, F., Elwell, C., & Csibra, G
(2008) Early cortical specialization for face-to-face communication in human infants. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences , 275(1653), 2803–2811. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Henning, A., & Striano, T
(2011) Infant and maternal sensitivity to interpersonal timing. Child Development , 82(3), 916–931. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Henning, A., Striano, T., & Lieven, E.V.M
(2005) Maternal speech to infants at 1 and 3 months of age. Infant Behavior and Development , 28(4), 519–536. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hepper, P.G., Scott, D., & Shahidullah, S
(1993) Newborn and fetal response to maternal voice. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology , 11, 147–153. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jaffe, J., Beebe, B., Feldstein, S., Crown, C.L., & Jasnow, M
(2001) Rhythms of dialogue in infancy: Coordinated timing in development. Monograph of the Society for Research in Child Development , 66(2, Serial No. 131).Google Scholar
Johnson, M.H., & Morton, J
(1991)  Biology and Cognitive Development: The Case of Face Recognition . Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Johnson, S., Slaughter, V., & Carey, S
(1998) Whose gaze will infants follow? The elicitation of gaze-following in 12-month-olds. Developmental Science , 1(2), 233–238. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kaye, K., & Charney, R
(1981) Conversational asymmetry between mothers and children. Journal of Child Language , 8(01), 35–49. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kaye, K
(1982)  The Mental and Social Life of Babies: How Parents Create Persons . Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Kaye, K., & Wells, A.J
(1980) Mothers’ jiggling and the burst. Pause pattern in neonatal feeding. Infant Behavior and Development , 3(0), 29–46. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Keller, H., & Schölmerich, A
(1987) Infant vocalizations and parental reactions during the first 4 months of life. Developmental Psychology , 23(1), 62–67. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kita, S
(2003)  Pointing: Where Language, Culture, and Cognition Meet . Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Kozak-Mayer, N., & Tronick, E.Z
(1985) Mothers’turn-giving signals and infant turn-taking in mother–infant interaction. In T.M. Field & N.A. Fox (Eds.), Social Perception in Infants (pp. 199–216). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Lamb, M.E., Bornstein, M.H., & Teti, D.M
(2002)  Development in Infancy (4th Ed). Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Lavelli, M., & Fogel, A
(2005) Developmental changes in the relationship between the infant’s attention and emotion during early face-to-face communication: The 2-month transition. Developmental Psychology , 41(1), 265–280. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Legerstee, M
(2005)  Infants’ Sense of People: Precursors to a Theory of Mind . Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Legerstee, M., & Markova, G
(2008) Variations in 10-month-old infant imitation of people and things. Infant Behavior and Development , 31(1), 81–91. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Legerstee, M., Pomerleau, A., Malcuit, G., & Feider, H
(1987) The development of infants’ responses to people and a doll: Implications for research in communication. Infant Behavior and Development , 10(1), 81–95. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Levinson, S.C
(1983)  Pragmatics . Cambridge: CUP.Google Scholar
Liszkowski, U
(2011) Three lines in the emergence of prelinguistic communication and social cognition. Journal of Cognitive Education and Psychology , 10(1), 32–43. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., Henning, A., Striano, T., & Tomasello, M
(2004) Twelve-month-olds point to share attention and interest. Developmental Science , 7(3), 297–307. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., Striano, T., & Tomasello, M
(2006) 12- and 18-month-olds point to provide information for others. Journal of Cognition and Development , 7(2), 173–187. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liszkowski, U., & Tomasello, M
(2011) Individual differences in social, cognitive, and morphological aspects of infant pointing. Cognitive Development , 26(1), 16–29. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Markova, G., & Legerstee, M
(2006) Contingency, imitation, and affect sharing: Foundations of infants’ social awareness. Developmental Psychology , 42(1), 132–141. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2008) How infants come to learn about the minds of others. Zero to Three , 28(5), 26–32.Google Scholar
Masataka, N
(2003)  The Onset of Language . Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Masur, E.F., Flynn, V., & Eichorst, D.L
(2005) Maternal responsive and directive behaviors and utterances as predictors of children’s lexical development. Journal of Child Language , 32, 63–91. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Matthews, D., Behne, T., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M
(2012) Origins of the human pointing gesture: A training study. Developmental Science , 15(6), 817–829. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McCune, L
(1992) First words: A dynamic systems view. In C.A. Ferguson, L. Menn, & C. Steol-Gammon (Eds.), Phonological Development: Models, Research, Implications (pp. 313–336). Parkon, MD: York.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A.N
(1995) Understanding the intentions of others: Re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Developmental Psychology , 31(5), 838–850. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007) The ‘like me’ framework for recognizing and becoming an intentional agent. Acta Psychologica , 124(1), 26–43. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meltzoff, A.N., & Gopnik, A
(1993) The role of imitation in understanding persons and developing a theory of mind. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg & D.J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding Other Minds: Perspectives from Autism (pp. 335–366). Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A.N., & Moore, M.K
(1994) Imitation, memory, and the representation of persons. Infant Behavior and Development , 17(1), 83–99. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1989) Imitation in newborn infants: Exploring the range of gestures imitated and the underlying mechanisms. Developmental Psychology , 25(6), 954–962. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Miller, C.L
(1988) Parents’ perceptions and attributions of infant vocal behaviour and development. First Language , 8(23), 125–141. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Moore, C., & Corkum, V
(1994) Social understanding at the end of the first year of life. Developmental Review , 14(4), 349–372. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Moore, C., & D’Entremont, B
(2001) Developmental changes in pointing as a function of attentional focus. Journal of Cognition and Development , 2(2), 109–129. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mundy, P., Block, J., Delgado, C., Pomares, Y., Van Hecke, A.V., & Parlade, M.V
(2007) Individual differences and the development of joint attention in infancy. Child Development , 78(3), 938–954. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mundy, P., & Newell, L
(2007) Attention, joint attention, and social cognition. Current Directions in Psychological Science , 16(5), 269–274. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Murray, L., & Trevarthen, C
(1985) Emotional regulations of interactions between two-month-olds and their mothers. In T.M. Field & N.A. Fox (Eds.), Social Perception in Infants (pp. 177–197). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Ninio, A. & Snow, C
(1996)  Pragmatic Development . Boulder, CO: Westview Press.Google Scholar
Panneton Cooper, R. & Aslin, R.N
(1990) Preference for infant-directed speech in the first month after birth. Child Development , 61(5), 1584–1595. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Papaeliou, C., Minadakis, G., & Cavouras, D
(2002) Acoustic patterns of infant vocalizations expressing emotions and communicative functions. J Speech Lang Hear Res , 45(2), 311–317. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Papaeliou, C.F., & Trevarthen, C
(2006) Prelinguistic pitch patterns expressing ‘communication’ and ‘apprehension’. Journal of Child Language , 33(01), 163–178. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Peña, M., Maki, A., Kovačić, D., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Koizumi, H., Bouquet, F., & Mehler, J
 (2003) Sounds and silence: An optical topography study of language recognition at birth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences , 100(20), 11702–11705. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Phillips, A.T., Wellman, H.M., & Spelke, E.S
(2002) Infants’ ability to connect gaze and emotional expression to intentional action. Cognition , 85(1), 53–78. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J
(1954)  The Origins of Intelligence in Children . New York, NY: Norton.Google Scholar
Reddy, V
(2008)  How Infants Know Minds . Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Rochat, P., Querido, J.G., & Striano, T
(1999) Emerging sensitivity to the timing and structure of protoconversation in early infancy. Developmental Psychology , 35(4), 950–957. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rommetveit, R
(1974)  On Message Structure: A Framework for the Study of Language and Communication . London: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Salomo, D., & Liszkowski, U
(2013) Sociocultural settings influence the emergence of prelinguistic deictic gestures. Child Development , 84(4), 1296–1307. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Scaife, M., & Bruner, J.S
(1975) The capacity for joint visual attention in the infant. Nature , 253(5489), 265–266. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Scarr, S
(1992) Developmental theories for the 1990s: Development and individual differences. Child Development , 63(1), 1–19. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schwier, C., van Maanen, C., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M
(2006) Rational imitation in 12 months olds. Infancy , 10(3), 303–311. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Searle, J.R
(1969)  Speech Acts . Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Senju, A., & Csibra, G
(2008) Gaze following in human infants depends on communicative signals. Current Biology , 18(9), 668–671. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shatz, M., & O’Reilly, A.W
(1990) Conversational or communicative skill? A reassessment of two-year-olds’ behaviour in miscommunication episodes. Journal of Child Language , 17(01), 131–146. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Snow, C.E
(1977) The development of conversation between mothers and babies. Journal of Child Language , 4(01), 1–22. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Southgate, V., Van Maanen, C., & Csibra, G
(2007) Infant Pointing: Communication to cooperate or communication to learn? Child Development , 78(3), 735–740. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sperber, D. & Wilson, D
(1995)  Relevance: Communication and Cognition . Second Edition. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Stern, D.N
(1985) Affect attunement. In J.D. Call, E. Galenson & R.L. Tyson (Eds.), Frontiers of Infant Psychiatry (Vol. 2, pp. 3–14). New York, NY: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Striano, T., & Stahl, D
(2005) Sensitivity to triadic attention in early infancy. Developmental Science , 8(4), 333–343. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Toda, S., & Fogel, A
(1993) Infant response to the still-face situation at 3 and 6 months. Developmental Psychology , 29(3), 532–538. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M
(2003)  Constructing a Language: A Usage Based Theory of Language Acquisition . Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
(2006) Rational imitation in 12-month-old infants Christiane Schwier, Catharine van Maanen, Malinda Carpenter. Infancy , 10(3), 303–311. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2008)  Origins of Human Communication . Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Carpenter, M
(2007) Shared intentionality. Developmental Science , 10(1), 121–125. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H
(2005) Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences , 28(5), 675–691. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Herrmann, E
(2010) Ape and human cognition: What’s the difference? Current Directions in Psychological Science , 19(1), 3–8. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tamis-LeMonda, C.S., Bornstein, M.H., & Baumwell, L
(2001) Maternal responsiveness and children’s achievement of language milestones. Child Development , 72, 748–767. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Topál, J., Gergely, G., Miklósi, Á., Erdőhegyi, Á., & Csibra, G
(2008) Infants’ perseverative search errors are induced by pragmatic misinterpretation. Science , 321(5897), 1831–1834. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Trevarthen, C
(1979) Communication and co-operation in early infancy: A description of primary intersubjectivity. In M. Bullowa (Ed.), Before Speech: The Beginnings of Human Communication (pp. 321–347). Cambridge: CUP.Google Scholar
Tronick, E.Z., Als, H., & Adamson, L
(1979) The communicative structure of early face-to face interactions. In M. Bullowa (Ed.), Before Speech: The Beginnings of Human Communication (pp. 349–372). Cambridge: CUP.Google Scholar
Vihman, M.M. & DePaolis, R.A
(1998) Perception and production in early vocal development: Evidence from the acquisition of accent. In M.C. Gruber, D. Higgins, K.S. Olson, & T. Wysocki (Eds.). Chicago Linguistic Society 34, 373–86.Google Scholar
Vihman, M.M., & Miller, R
(1988) Words and babble at the threshold of language acquisition. In M.D. Smith & J. Locke (Eds.), The Emergent Lexicon: The Child’s Development of a Linguistic Vocabulary (pp. 151–184). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Vouloumanos, A., & Werker, J.F
(2007) Listening to language at birth: Evidence for a bias for speech in neonates. Developmental Science , 10(2), 159–164. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wittgenstein, L
(1953)  Philosophical Investigations . Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Woodward, A.L
(1999) Infants’ ability to distinguish between purposeful and non-purposeful behaviors. Infant Behavior and Development , 22(2), 145–160. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zeifman, D.M
(2001) An ethological analysis of human infant crying: Answering Tinbergen’s four questions. Developmental Psychobiology , 39(4), 265–285. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 3 other publications

Dosi, Ifigeneia & Savvas Sotiriadis
2020. Interventions for early language development in monolingual and bilingual children with autism spectrum disorders: Two case studies. International Journal of Research Studies in Education 9:7 DOI logo
Roche, Laura, Amarie Carnett, Jeff Sigafoos, Michelle Stevens, Mark F. O’Reilly, Giulio E. Lancioni & Peter B. Marschik
2019. Using a Textual Prompt to Teach Multiword Requesting to Two Children With Autism Spectrum Disorder. Behavior Modification 43:6  pp. 819 ff. DOI logo
Vicente, Agustín & Ingrid Lossius Falkum
2023. Accounting for the preference for literal meanings in autism spectrum conditions. Mind & Language 38:1  pp. 119 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 3 march 2023. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.