Article published in:
Pragmatic Development in First Language Acquisition
Edited by Danielle Matthews
[Trends in Language Acquisition Research 10] 2014
► pp. 161182
References
Allen, S.
(2000) A discourse-pragmatic explanation for argument representation in child Inuktitut. Linguistics , 38(3), 483–521. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Allen, S., Skarabela, B., & Hughes, M.
(2008) Using corpora to examine discourse effects in syntax. In H. Behrens (Ed.), Corpora in Language Acquisition Research: Finding Structure in Data (pp. 99–137). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Anselmi, D., Tomasello, M. & Acunzo, M.
(1986) Young children’s responses to neutral and specific contingent queries. Journal of Child Language , 13, 135–44. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ariel, M.
(1990)  Accessing Noun Phrase Antecedents . London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Arnold, J.E., Bennetto, L., & Diehl, J.
(2009) Reference production in young speakers with and without autism: Effects of discourse status and processing constraints. Cognition , 110(2), 131–46. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Arnold, J., Brown-Schmidt, S. & Trueswell, J.
(2007) Children’s use of gender and order-of-mention during pronoun comprehension. Language and Cognitive Processes , 22(4), 527–565. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Baker, N.D., & Greenfield, P.M.
(1988) The development of new and old information in young children’s early language. Language Sciences , 10(1), 3–34. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Baltaxe, C.
(1977) Pragmatic deficits in the language of autistic adolescents. Journal of Pediatric Psychology , 2, 176–180. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bahtiyar, S., & Kuntay, A.C.
(2009) Integration of communicative partner’s visual perspective in patterns of referential requests. Journal of Child Language , 36, 529–54. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Begeer, S., Malle, B.F., Nieuwland, M.S., & Keysar, B.
(2010) Using Theory of Mind to represent and take part in social interactions: Comparing individuals with high-functioning autism and typically developing controls. European Journal of Developmental Psychology , 7(1), 104–122. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Brener, R.
(1983) Learning the deictic meaning of third person pronouns. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research , 12, 235–262.Google Scholar
Brown, R., & Fraser, C.
(1963) The acquisition of syntax. In Cofer, C.N., Musgrave, B. (Eds). Verbal Behaviour and Learning: Problems and Processes . New York, NY: McGraw-Hill. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Charman, T., & Shmueli-Goetz, Y.
(1998) The relationship between theory of mind, language, and narrative discourse: An experimental study. Current Psychology of Cognition , 17, 245–271.Google Scholar
Chafe, W.
(1976) Givenness, contrastiveness, definiteness, subjects, topics, & point of view. In C.N. Li (Ed.), Subject and Topic (pp. 25–56). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
(1994)  Discourse, Consciousness, and Time: The Flow and Displacement of Conscious Experience in Speaking and Writing . Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Chien, Y.C., & Wexler, K.
(1990) Children’s knowledge of locality conditions in binding as evidence for the modularity of syntax and pragmatics. Language Acquisition: A Journal of Developmental Linguistics , 1, 225–295. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Clancy, P.M.
(1993) Preferred argument structure in Korean acquisition. In E.V. Clark (Ed.), Proceedings of the 25th Annual Child Language Research Forum (pp. 307–314). Stanford, CA: CSLI.Google Scholar
(2003) The lexicon in interaction: Developmental origins of preferred argument structure in Korean. In J. Du Bois, E. Kumpf, & W.J. Ashby (Eds.), Preferred Argument Structure: Grammar as Architecture for Function (pp. 81–108). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Clark, H.H., & Schaefer, E.F.
(1987) Collaborating on contributions to conversation. Language and Cognitive Processes , 2, 19–41. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Clark, H.H., & Wilkes-Gibbs, D.
(1986) Referring as a collaborative process. Cognition , 22, 1–39. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Clark, H.H., Schreuder, R., & Buttrick, S.
(1983) Common ground and the understanding of demonstrative reference. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior , 22, 245–258. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Clark, H.H., & Bangerter, A.
(2004) Changing ideas about reference. In I.A. Noveck & D. ­Sperber (Eds.), Experimental Pragmatics (pp. 25–49). Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
Dahlgren, S., & Sandberg, D.A.
(2008) Referential communication in children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism: The International Journal of Research and Practice , 12(4), 335–48. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Davies, C., & Katsos, N.
(2010) Over-informative children: Production/comprehension asymmetry or tolerance to pragmatic violations? Lingua , 120(8), 1956–1972. Special Issue on Asymmetries in Child Language . CrossrefGoogle Scholar
De Cat, C.
(2011) Information tracking and encoding in early L1: Linguistic competence vs. cognitive limitations. Journal of Child Language , 38(4), 828–860. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Deutsch, W., & Pechmann, T.
(1982) Social-interaction and the development of definite descriptions. Cognition , 11(2), 159–184. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
de Villiers, J., Stainton, R.J. & Szatmari, P.
(2007) Pragmatic abilities in autism spectrum disorder: A case study in philosophy and the empirical. Midwest Studies in Philosophy , 31(1), 292–316. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Evans, K.E., & Demuth, K.
(2011) Individual differences in pronoun reversal: Evidence from two longitudinal case studies. Journal of Child Language, 39(1), 1–30.Google Scholar
Ford, W., & Olson, D.
(1975) The elaboration of the noun phrase in children’s description of objects. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology , 19(3), 371–382. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Girbau, D.
(2001) Children’s referential communication failure: The ambiguity and abbreviation of messages. Journal of Language and Social Psychology , 20(1–2), 81–89. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Glucksberg, S. & Krauss, R.
(1967) What do people say after they have learned how to talk? Studies of development and referential communication. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly , 13, 309–316.Google Scholar
Glucksberg, S., Krauss, R.M., & Higgins, E.T.
(1975) The development of referential communication skills. In F.D. Horowitz (Ed.), Review of Child Development Vol. 4 . Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Graf, E., Theakston, A., Lieven, E. & Tomasello, M.
in press). Subject and object omission in children’s early transitive constructions: A discourse-pragmatic approach. Applied Psycholinguistics .
Grice, H.P.
(1975) Logic and conversation. In P. Cole & J. Morgan (Eds.), Syntax and Semantics, 3: Speech Acts (pp. 41–58). New York, NY: Academic Press. Reprinted in Grice, H.P. (1989). Studies in the Way of Words (pp. 22–40). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Guerriero, S., Oshima-Takane, Y., & Kuriyama, Y.
(2006) The development of referential choice in English and Japanese: A discourse-pragmatic perspective. Journal of Child Language , 33, 823–57. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gundel, J.K., Hedberg, N., & Zacharski, R.
(1993) Cognitive status and the form of referring expressions in discourse. Language , 69(2), 274–307. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hughes, M., & Allen, S.
(2013) The effect of individual discourse pragmatic features on referential choice in child English. Journal of Pragmatics . http://​dx​.doi​.org​/10​.1016​/j​.pragma​.2013​.05​.005
Kanner, L.
(1943) Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child , 2, 217–250.Google Scholar
(1946) Irrelevant and metaphorical language. American Journal of Psychiatry , 103, 242–246.Google Scholar
Keysar, B., Barr, D.J., Balin, J.A., & Brauner, J.S.
(2000) Taking perspective in conversation: The role of mutual knowledge in comprehension. Psychological Science , 11(1), 32–38. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lee, A., Hobson, R.P., & Chiat, S.
(1994) I, you, me and autism: An experimental study. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders , 24, 155–176. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Liebal, K., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M.
(2010) Infants’ use of shared experience in declarative pointing. Infancy , 15, 545–556. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Liebal, K., Behne, T., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M.
(2009) Infants use shared experience to interpret pointing gestures. Developmental Science , 12(2), 264–271. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Liszkowski, U.
(2008) Before L1: A differentiated perspective on infant gestures. Gesture , 8(2), 180–196. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Loveland, K.
(1984) Learning about points of view: Spatial perspective and the acquisition of “I/you”. Journal of Child Language , 11, 535–556. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Loveland, K., Tunali, B., Kelley, M.L., & McEvoy, R.E.
(1989) Referential communication and response adequacy in autism and Down’s syndrome. Applied Psycholinguistics , 10, 301–313. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Matthews, D., Butcher, J., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M.
(2012) Two- and four-year-olds learn to adapt referring expressions to context: Effects of distracters and feedback on referential communication. TopiCS , 4(2), 184–210.Google Scholar
Matthews, D., Theakston, A., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M.
(2009) Pronoun co-referencing errors: Challenges for generativist and usage-based accounts. Cognitive Linguistics , 20(3), 599–626. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Matthews, D., Lieven, E., Theakston, A., & Tomasello, M.
(2006) The effect of perceptual availability and prior discourse on young children’s use of referring expressions. Applied Psycholinguistics , 27, 403–422. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Matthews, D., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M.
(2007) How toddlers and preschoolers learn to uniquely identify referents for others: A training study. Child Development , 78(6), 1744–1759. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Maratsos, M.
(1974) Preschool children’s use of definite and indefinite articles. Child Development , 45, 446–455. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Menig-Peterson, C.L.
(1975) The modification of communicative behaviour in preschool-aged children as a function of listener’s perspective. Child Development , 46, 1015–1018. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Moll, H., & Tomasello, M.
(2006) Level 1 perspective-taking at 24 months of age. British Journal of Developmental Psychology , 24, 603–613. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2007) How 14- and 18-month-olds know what others have experienced. Developmental Psychology , 43(2), 309–317. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Morisseau, T., Davies, C., & Matthews, D.
(2013) How do 3- and 5-year-olds respond to over- and under-informative utterances? Journal of Pragmatics . http://​dx​.doi​.org​/10​.1016​/j​.pragma​.2013​.03​.007
Nadig, A. & Sedivy, J.
(2002) Evidence of perspective-taking constraints in children’s on-line reference resolution. Psychological Science , 13(4), 329–336. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
NadigA., Vivanti, G., & Ozonoff, S.
(2009) Adaptation of object descriptions to a partner under increasing communicative demands: A comparison of children with and without autism. Autism Research, 2(6): 334–347.Google Scholar
Narasimhan, B., Budwig, N., & Murty, L.
(2005) Argument realization in Hindi caregiver-child discourse. Journal of Pragmatics , 37, 461–95. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, B., Adamson, L., & Bakeman, R.
(2008) Toddlers’ joint engagement experience facilitates preschoolers’ acquisition of Theory of Mind. Developmental Science , 11(6), 847–852. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nilsen, E.S., & Mangal, L.
in press). Which is important for preschoolers’ production and repair of statements: What the listener knows or what the listener says? Journal of Child Language .
Nilsen, E., & Fecica, A.M.
(2011) A model of communicative perspective-taking for typical and atypical populations of children. Developmental Review , 31, 55–78. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nilsen, E., & Graham, S.
(2009) The relations between children’s communicative perspective-taking and executive functioning. Cognitive Psychology , 58, 220–249. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nilsen, E., Graham, S., Smith, S., & Chambers, C.
(2008) Preschoolers’ sensitivity to referential ambiguity: Evidence for a dissociation between implicit understanding and explicit behavior. Developmental Science , 11(4), 556–562. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Norbury, C.F. & Bishop, D.V.M.
(2003) Narrative skills of children with communication impairments. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders , 38, 287–313. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
O’Neill, D.K. & Topolovec, J.
(2001) Two-year-old children’s sensitivity to the referential (in)efficacy of their own pointing gestures. Journal of Child Language , 28, 1–28. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
O’Neill, D.K.
(1996) Two-year-old children’s sensitivity to a parent’s knowledge state when making requests. Child Development , 67, 695–677. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J. & Navarro, S.
(2003) Subject realization and crosslinguistic interference in the bilingual acquisition of Spanish and English. Journal of Child Language , 30, 1–23. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Peterson, C., & McCabe, A.
(1994) A social interactionist account of developing decontextualized narrative skill. Developmental Psychology , 30(6), 937–948. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pyykkönen, P., Matthews, D., & Järvikivi, J.
(2010) Verb semantics affects children’s pronoun comprehension: Evidence form eye-movements. Language and Cognitive Processes , 25(1), 115–129. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, R., & Patterson, C.
(1983) Perspective taking and referential communication: The question of correspondence reconsidered. Child Development , 54, 1005–1014. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, E., & Robinson, W.
(1982) The advancement of children’s verbal referential communication skills: The role of metacognitive guidance. International Journal of Behavioral Development , 5, 329–355. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Salomo, D., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M.
(2013) Children’s ability to answer different types of questions. Journal of Child Language , 40(2), 469–491. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Saylor, M.M., Baird, J.A., & Gallerani, C.
(2006) Telling others what’s new: Preschoolers’ adherence to the given-new contract. Journal of Cognition and Development , 7(3), 341–379. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Serratrice, L., Sorace, A., & Paoli, S.
(2004) Crosslinguistic influence at the syntax-pragmatics interface: Subjects and objects in English-Italian bilingual and monolingual acquisition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition , 7(3), 183–205. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Serratrice, L.
(2005) The role of discourse pragmatics in the acquisition of subjects in Italian. Applied Psycholinguistics , 26(3), 437–462. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2007) Null and overt subjects at the syntax-discourse interface: Evidence from monolingual and bilingual acquisition. In J. van Kampen, S. Bauw, & M. Pinto (Eds.), Selected Papers from The Romance Turn II. LOT Occasional Series (pp. 181–200). Utrecht: LOT.Google Scholar
Song, H., & Fisher, C.
(2007) Discourse prominence effects on 2.5-year-old children’s interpretation of pronouns. Lingua , 117, 1959–1987. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2005) Who’s “she”? Discourse structure influences preschoolers’ pronoun interpretation. Journal of Memory and Language , 52, 29–47. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sonnenschein, S., & Whitehurst, G.J.
(1984) Developing referential communication – A hierarchy of skills. Child Development , 55(5), 1936–1945. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sonnenschein, S.
(1988) The development of referential communication – Speaking to different listeners. Child Development , 59(3), 694–702. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Skarabela, B.
(2007) Signs of early social cognition in children’s syntax: The case of joint attention in argument realization in child Inuktitut. Lingua , 117(11), 1837–1857. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H.
(1993) ‘What language reveals about the understanding of minds in children with autism’. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D.J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding Other Minds: Perspectives from Autism (pp. 138–157). Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
(1994) ‘Dissociations in form and function in the acquisition of language by autistic children’. In H. Tager-Flusberg (Ed.), Constraints on Language Acquisition: Studies of Atypical Children (pp. 175–194). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
(1995) ‘Once upon a ribbit’: Stories narrated by autistic children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology , 13, 45–59. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2001) A re-examination of the theory of mind hypothesis of autism. In J. Burack, T. Charman, N. Yirmiya, & P. Zelazo (Eds.), The Development of Autism: Perspectives from Theory and Research (pp. 173–193). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Volden, J., Mulcahy, R.F., & Holdgrafer, G.
(1997) Pragmatic language disorder and perspective taking in autistic speakers. Applied Psycholinguistics , 18(02): 181. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Whitehurst, G.J.
(1976) Development of communication – changes with age and modeling. Child Development , 47(2), 473–482. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Whitehurst, G.J., & Sonnenschein, S.
(1981) The development of informative messages in referential communication: Knowing when vs. knowing how. In W.P. Dickson (Ed.), Children’s Oral Communication Skills . New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Wykes, T.
(1981) Inference and children’s comprehension of pronouns. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology , 32, 264–278. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 1 other publications

Malkin, Louise, Kirsten Abbot-Smith & David Williams
2018. Is verbal reference impaired in autism spectrum disorder? A systematic review. Autism & Developmental Language Impairments 3  pp. 239694151876316 ff. Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 26 february 2021. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.