Article published in:
Language in Interaction: Studies in honor of Eve V. Clark
Edited by Inbal Arnon, Marisa Casillas, Chigusa Kurumada and Bruno Estigarribia
[Trends in Language Acquisition Research 12] 2014
► pp. 175190
References
Baldwin, D.A
(1991) Infants’ contribution to the achievement of joint reference. Child Development , 62(5), 875–890. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Barr, D.J., & Keysar, B
(2002) Anchoring comprehension in linguistic precedents. Journal of Memory and Language , 46(2), 391–418. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bortfeld, H., & Brennan, S.E
(1997) Use and acquisition of idiomatic expressions in referring by native and non-native speakers. Discourse Processes , 23(2), 119–147. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Brennan, S.E., & Clark, H.H
(1996) Conceptual pacts and lexical choice in conversation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition , 22(6), 1482–1493. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Brennan, S.E., Galati, A., & Kuhlen, A.K
(2010) Two minds, one dialog: Coordinating speaking and understanding. Psychology of Learning and Motivation , 53, 301–344. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Brennan, S.E., & Hanna, J.E
(2009) Partner-specific adaptation in dialog. Topics in Cognitive Science , 1(2), 274–291. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Brown, P.M., & Dell, G.S
(1987) Adapting production to comprehension: The explicit mention of instruments. Cognitive Psychology , 19(4), 441–472. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Brown-Schmidt, S
(2009) Partner-specific interpretation of maintained referential precedents during interactive dialog. Journal of Memory and Language , 61(2), 171–190. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Callanan, M.A., & Sabbagh, M.A
(2004) Multiple labels for objects in conversations with young children: Parents’ language and children’s developing expectations about word meanings. Developmental Psychology , 40(5), 746–763. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Clark, E.V
(1987) The principle of contrast: A constraint on language acquisition. In B. MacWhinney (Ed.) Mechanisms of language acquisition (pp. 1–33). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
(1988) On the logic of contrast. Journal of Child Language , 15(2), 317–335. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1990) On the pragmatics of contrast. Journal of Child Language , 17(2), 417–431. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1993)  The lexicon in acquisition . Cambridge: CUP. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1997) Conceptual perspective and lexical choice in acquisition. Cognition , 64(1), 1–37. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2009)  First language acquisition . Cambridge: CUP. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2007) Conventionality and contrast in language and language acquisition. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development , 115, 11–23. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Clark, E.V., & Svaib, T.A
(1997) Speaker perspective and reference in young children. First Language , 17(51), 57–74. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Clark, H.H., & Wilkes-Gibbs, D
(1986) Referring as a collaborative process. Cognition , 22(1), 1–39. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Clark, H.H., and Brennan, S.A
. (1991) Grounding in communication. In L.B. Resnick, J.M. Levine, & S.D. Teasley (Eds.). Perspectives on socially shared cognition (pp. 127–149). Washington: APA Books CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Danet, B
(1980) ‘Baby’ or ‘fetus’?: Language and the construction of reality in a manslaughter trial. Semiotica , 32(3–4), 187––220. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Deák, G.O., Yen, L., & Pettit, J
(2001) By any other name: When will preschoolers produce several labels for a referent? Journal of Child Language , 28(3), 787–804. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Diesendruck, G
(2005) The principles of conventionality and contrast in word learning: An empirical examination. Developmental Psychology , 41(3), 451–463. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Diesendruck, G., & Markson, L
(2001) Children’s avoidance of lexical overlap: A pragmatic account. Developmental Psychology , 37(5), 630–641. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Doherty, M.J
(2000) Children’s understanding of homonymy: Metalinguistic awareness and false belief. Journal of Child Language , 27(2), 367–392. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fisher, C., Church, B.A., & Chambers, K.E
(2004) Learning to identify spoken words. In D.G. Hall & S. Waxman (Eds.), Weaving a lexicon (pp. 3–40). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Fowler, C.A., Brown, J.M., Sabadini, L., & Weihing, J
(2003) Rapid access to speech gestures in perception: Evidence from choice and simple response time tasks. Journal of Memory and Language , 49(3), 396–413. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fremery, J., & Grassman, S
In prep). Do 4-year-olds choose referential expressions according to speaker-specific referential pacts ?
Galati, A., & Brennan, S.E
(2010) Attenuating information in spoken communication: For the speaker, or for the addressee? Journal of Memory and Language , 62(1), 35–51. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Garrod, S., & Anderson, A
(1987) Saying what you mean in dialogue: A study in conceptual and semantic co-ordination. Cognition , 27(2), 181–218. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Geeraerts, D., Grondelaers, S., & Bakema, P
(1994)  The structure of lexical variation: Meaning, naming, and context , Vol. 5. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Goldinger, S.D
(1998) Echoes of echoes? An episodic theory of lexical access. Psychological Review , 105(2), 251–279. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Graham, S.A., Sedivy, J., & Khu, M
(2013) That’s not what you said earlier: Preschoolers expect partners to be referentially consistent. Journal of Child Language, FirstView , 1–17.Google Scholar
Huygens, C., & der Wetenschappen, H.M
(1893)  Oeuvres complétes de Christiaan Huygens: Martinus Nijhoff.Google Scholar
Köymen,B.,Schmerse,D.,Lieven,E., &Tomasello, M
Young children create partner-specific referential pacts with peers.
Krauss, R.M., & Weinheimer, S
(1966) Concurrent feedback, confirmation, and the encoding of referents in verbal communication. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology , 4(3), 343–346. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kronmüller, E., & Barr, D.J
(2007) Perspective-free pragmatics: Broken precedents and the recovery-from-preemption hypothesis. Journal of Memory and Language , 56(3), 436–455. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Levelt, W.J.M., & Kelter, S
(1982) Surface form and memory in question answering. Cognitive Psychology , 14(1), 78–106. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Levinson, S.C
(1995) Interactional biases in human thinking. In E.N. Goody (Ed.), Social intelligence and interaction: Expressions and implications of the social bias in human intelligence (pp. 221–260). Cambridge: CUP. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lockridge, C., & Brennan, S
(2002) Addressees’ needs influence speakers’ early syntactic choices. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review , 9(3), 550–557. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Matthews, D., Butcher, J., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M
(2012) Two- and four-year-olds learn to adapt referring expressions to context: Effects of distracters and feedback on referential communication. Topics in Cognitive Science , 4(2), 184–210. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Matthews, D., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M
(2010) What’s in a manner of speaking? Children’s sensitivity to partner-specific referential precedents. Developmental Psychology , 46(4), 749–760. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Metzing, C., & Brennan, S.E
(2003) When conceptual pacts are broken: Partner-specific effects on the comprehension of referring expressions. Journal of Memory and Language , 49(2), 201–213. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nilsen, E.S., Graham, S.A., Smith, S., & Chambers, C.G
(2008) Preschoolers’ sensitivity to referential ambiguity: Evidence for a dissociation between implicit understanding and explicit behavior. Developmental Science , 11(4), 556–562. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nurmsoo, E., Robinson, E., & Butterfill, S
(2010) Children’s selective learning from others. Review of Philosophy and Psychology , 1(4), 551–561. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Perner, J., Rendl, B., & Garnham, A
(2007) Objects of desire, thought, and reality: Problems of anchoring discourse referents in development. Mind & Language , 22(5), 475–513. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Perner, J., Stummer, S., Sprung, M., & Doherty, M
(2002) Theory of mind finds its Piagetian perspective: Why alternative naming comes with understanding belief. Cognitive Development , 17(3–4), 1451–1472. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pickering, M.J., & Ferreira, V.S
(2008) Structural priming: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin , 134(3), 427–459. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pickering, M.J., & Garrod, S
(2004) Toward a mechanistic psychology of dialogue. Behavioral and Brain Sciences , 27(2), 169–190.Google Scholar
Rakoczy, H., Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M
(2008) The sources of normativity: Young children’s awareness of the normative structure of games. Developmental Psychology , 44(3), 875–881. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Richardson, D.C., & Dale, R
(2005) Looking to understand: The coupling between speakers’ and listeners’ eye movements and its relationship to discourse comprehension. Cognitive Science , 29(6), 1045–1060. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sabbagh, M.A., & Baldwin, D.A
(2001) Learning words from knowledgeable versus ignorant speakers: Links between preschoolers’ theory of mind and semantic development. Child Development , 72(4), 1054–1070. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schelling, T.C
(1980)  The strategy of conflict . Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Schiffer, S.R
(1972)  Meaning . Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Schober, M.F
(1993) Spatial perspective-taking in conversation. Cognition , 47(1), 1–24. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schober, M.F., & Clark, H.H
(1989) Understanding by addressees and overhearers. Cognitive Psychology , 21(2), 211–232. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Scofield, J., & Behrend, D.A
(2007) Two-year-olds differentially disambiguate novel words and facts. Journal of Child Language , 34(4), 875–889. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Shintel, H., & Keysar, B
(2007) You said it before and you’ll say it again: Expectations of consistency in communication. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition , 33(2), 357–369. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2009) Less is more: A minimalist account of joint action in communication. Topics in Cognitive Science , 1(2), 260–273. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sobel, D.M., Sedivy, J., Buchanan, D.W., & Hennessy, R
(2012) Speaker reliability in preschoolers’ inferences about the meanings of novel words. Journal of Child Language , 39(1), 90–104. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Southgate, V., Chevallier, C., & Csibra, G
(2010) Seventeen-month-olds appeal to false beliefs to interpret others’ referential communication. Developmental Science , 13(6), 907–912. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Stellmann, P., & Brennan, S
(1993) Flexible perspective-setting in conversation. Paper presented at the 34th Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Washington, DC.
Van Der Wege, M.M
(2009) Lexical entrainment and lexical differentiation in reference phrase choice. Journal of Memory and Language , 60(4), 448–463. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wyman, E., Rakoczy, H., & Tomasello, M
(2009) Young children understand multiple pretend identities in their object play. British Journal of Developmental Psychology , 27(2), 385–404. CrossrefGoogle Scholar