Part of
Language in Interaction: Studies in honor of Eve V. Clark
Edited by Inbal Arnon, Marisa Casillas, Chigusa Kurumada and Bruno Estigarribia
[Trends in Language Acquisition Research 12] 2014
► pp. 191206
References (39)
References
Akhtar, N. (2005). The robustness of learning through overhearing. Developmental Science , 8, 199–209. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Au, T., & Glusman, M. (1990). The principle of mutual exclusivity in word learning: To honor or not to honor? Child Development , 61, 1474–1490. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E. (1988). Levels of bilingualism and levels of linguistic awareness. Developmental Psychology , 24, 560–567. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Callanan, M.A., & Sabbagh, M.A. (2004). Multiple labels for objects in conversations with young children: Parents’ language and children’s developing expectations about word meanings. Developmental Psychology , 40, 746–763. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, E.V. (1978). Awareness of language: Some evidence from what children say and do. In A. Sinclair, R. Jarvella, & W.J.M. Levelt (Eds.), The child’s conception of language (pp. 17–43). New York, NY: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2004). Pragmatics and language acquisition. In L.R. Horn & G. Ward (Eds.), Handbook of pragmatics (pp. 562–577). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
. (2007). Young children’s uptake of new words in conversation. Language in Society , 36, 157–182. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, E.V., & Amaral, P. (2010). Children build on pragmatic information in language acquisition. Language and Linguistics Compass , 4, 445–457. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, E.V., & Bernicot, J. (2008). Repetition as ratification: How parents and children place information in common ground. Journal of Child Language , 35, 349–371. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, E.V., & Estigarribia, B. (2011). Using speech and gesture to introduce new objects to young children. Gesture, 11 , 1–23. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, E.V., Gelman, S.A., & Lane, N.M. (1985). Noun compounds and category structure in young children. Child Development , 56, 84–94. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, E.V., & Sengul, C.J. (1978). Strategies in the acquisition of deixis. Journal of Child Language , 5, 457–475. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
De Houwer, A. (1990). The acquisition of two languages: A case study . Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Deuchar, M., & Quay, S. (1999). Language choice in the earliest utterances: A case study with methodological implications. Journal of Child Language , 26 , 461–475. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Diesendruck, G. (2005). The principles of conventionality and contrast in word learning: An empirical examination. Developmental Psychology , 41, 451–463. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Genesee, F., Boivin, I., & Nicoladis, E. (1996). Talking with strangers: A study of bilingual children’s communicative competence. Applied Psycholinguistics , 17, 427–442. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jaswal, V.K. (2004). Don’t believe everything you hear: Preschoolers’ sensitivity to speaker intent in category induction. Child Development , 75, 1871–1885. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kasuya, H. (1998). Determinants of language choice in bilingual children: The role of input. International Journal of Bilingualism , 2, 327–346.Google Scholar
Koppe, R., & Meisel, J.M. (1995). Code-switching in bilingual first language acquisition. In L. Milroy & P. Muysken (Eds.), One speaker, two languages: Cross-disciplinary perspectives on code-switching (pp. 276–301). Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lanza, E. (2001). Bilingual first language acquisition: A discourse perspective on language contact in parent-child interaction. In J. Cenoz & F. Genesee (Eds.), Trends in bilingual acquisition (pp. 201–230). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mehler, J., Jusczyk, P., Lambertz, G., Halsted, N., Bertoncini, J., & Amiel-Tison, C. (1988). A precursor of language acquisition in young infants. Cognition , 29, 143–178. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nicoladis, E., & Genesee, F. (1996). A longitudinal study of pragmatic differentiation in young bilingual children. Language Learning , 46, 439–464. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Oller, D.K., Pearson, B.Z. & Cobo-Lewis, A.B. (2007). Profile effects in early bilingual language and literacy. Applied Psycholinguistics , 28, 191–230. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pan, B.A. (1995). Code negotiation in bilingual families: “My body starts speaking English.” Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development , 16, 315–327. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J., & Genesee, F. (1996). Syntactic acquisition in bilingual children: Autonomous or interdependent? Studies in Second Language Acquisition , 18, 1–25. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pearson, B.Z., Fernandez, S., & Oller, D.K. (1995). Cross-language synonyms in the lexicons of bilingual infants: one language or two? Journal of Child Language , 22, 345–368. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Quay, S. (2008). Dinner conversations with a trilingual two-year-old: Language socialization in a multilingual context. First Language , 28, 5–33. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2012). Discourse practices of trilingual mothers: Effects on minority home language development in Japan. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism , 15, 435–453. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rosenblum, T., & Pinker, S.A. (1983). Word magic revisited: Monolingual and bilingual children’s understanding of the word-object relationship. Child Development , 54, 773–780. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sabbagh, M., & Baldwin, D. (2001). Learning words from knowledgeable versus ignorant speakers: Links between preschoolers’ theory of mind and semantic development. Child Development , 72, 1054–1070. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shatz, M., & Gelman, R. (1973). The development of communication skills: Modifications in the speech of young children as a function of listener. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development , 38, 1–37. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Siegel, M., & Surian, L. (2007). Conversational understanding in young children. In E. Hoff & M. Shatz (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of language development (pp. 304–323). Oxford: Blackwell. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tare, M., & Gelman, S.A. (2010). Can you say it another way? Cognitive factors in bilingual children’s pragmatic language skills. Journal of Cognition and Development , 11, 137–158. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2011). Bilingual parents’ modeling of pragmatic language use in multiparty interactions. Applied Psycholinguistics , 32, 761–780. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2001). Perceiving intentions and learning words in the second year of life. In M. Bowerman & S.C. Levinson (Eds.), Language acquisition and conceptual development (pp. 132–158). Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vedder, P., Kook, H., & Muysken, P. (1996). Language choice and functional differentiation of languages in bilingual parent-child reading. Applied Psycholinguistics , 17, 461–484. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wali, K. (2005). Marathi . Munich: Lincom.Google Scholar
Wellman, H.M., & Liu, D. (2004). Scaling of theory of mind tasks. Child Development , 75, 523–541. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yan, S., & Nicoladis, E. (2009). Finding le mot juste: Differences between bilingual and monolingual children’s lexical access in comprehension and production. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition , 12, 323–335. DOI logoGoogle Scholar