References
Adam, G. & Bat-El, O
2008Segmental effects on syllable selection: Evidence from Hebrew. In Language Acquisition and Development. Proceedings of GALA 2007, A. Gavarró & M.J. Freitas (eds), 1-11. Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars.Google Scholar
Allen, G.D. & Hawkins, S
1978The development of phonological rhythm. In Syllables and Segments, A. Bell & J.B. Hooper (eds), 173-185. Amsterdam: North-Holland.Google Scholar
1980Phonological rhythm: Definition and development. Child Phonology 1: 227-256. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Becker, M
2003Hebrew stress: Can’t you hear those trochees? In Proceedings of the 26th Penn Linguistics Colloquium, E. Kaiser & S. Arunachalam (eds), 45-58. Philadelphia PA: University of Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
Ben-David, A
2001Language Acquisition and Phonological Theory: Universal and Variable Processes Across Children and Across Languages. PhD dissertation, Tel Aviv University.Google Scholar
Ben-David, A. & Berman, R.A
2007Israeli Hebrew Speech Acquisition. In The International Guide to Speech Acquisition, S. McLeod (ed), 437-456. New York NY: Thomson Delmar Learning.Google Scholar
Berman, R.A
1977Natural phonological processes at the one-word stage. Lingua 43: 1-21. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
1978Modern Hebrew Structure. Tel Aviv: University Publishing Projects.Google Scholar
Best, C. & McRoberts G
2003Infant perception of nonnative consonant contrasts that adults assimilate in different ways. Language and Speech 46: 183-216. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bhaya Nair, R
1991Monosyllabic English or disyllabic Hindi? Indian Linguistics 52: 51-90.Google Scholar
Bolozky, S
1982Remarks on rhythmic stress in Modern Hebrew. Linguistics 18: 275-89. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boysson-Bardies, B. & Vihman, M.M
1991Adaptation to language: Evidence from babbling and first words in four languages. Language 67: 267-319. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cohen, E-G
2012Vowel harmony and universality in Hebrew acquisition. Brill’s Annual of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics 4: 7-29. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Colombo, J. & Bundy, R.S
1981A method for the measurement of infant auditory selectivity. Infant Behavior and Development 4: 219-223. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cooper, R.P., Abraham, J. & Berman, S
1997The development of infants' preference for motherese. Infant Behavior and Development 20: 477-488. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cooper, R.P. & Aslin, R.N
1990Preference for infant-directed speech in the first month after birth. Child Development 61: 1584-1595. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
1994Developmental differences in infant attention to the spectral properties of infant-directed speech. Child Development 65: 1663-1677. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dar, M., Vihman, M.M. & Keren-Portnoy, T
under revision. An order effect in English infants’ response to an Urdu voiceless affricate contrast.
Engstrand, O., Williams, K. & Lacerda, F
2003Does babbling sound native? Listener responses to vocalizations produced by Swedish and American 12- and 18-month olds. Phonetica 60: 17-44. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fernald, A
1985Four-month-old infants prefer to listen to motherese. Infant Behavior and Development 8: 181-195. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fernald, A. & Kuhl, P.K
1987Acoustic determinants of infant preference for motherese speech. Infant Behavior and Development 10: 279-293. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Floccia, C., Keren-Portnoy, T., DePaolis, R., Duffy, H., Delle Luche, C., Durrant, S., White, L., Goslin, J. & Vihman, M
2016A robust failure to replicate word segmentation findings in British English infants. Cognition 148: 1-9. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Friederici, A.D., Friedrich, M. & Christophe, A
2007Brain responses in 4-month-old infants are already language specific. Current Biology 17: 1208-1211 DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Friedlander, S.S. & Wisdom, Z.B
1971Pre-verbal infants’ selective operant responses for different levels of auditory complexity and linguistic redundancy. Paper presented at the Annual General Meeting of the Eastern Psychological Association, New York NY.
Gafni, C
2012Child consonant harmony: Identification and properties. Brill’s Annual of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics 4: 30-54. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Glenn, S.M., Cunningham, C.C. & Joyce, P.F
1981A study of auditory preferences on nonhandicapped infants with Down’s syndrome. Child Development 52: 1303-1307. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Glinert, L
1989The Grammar of Modern Hebrew. Cambridge: CUP.Google Scholar
Goffman, L., Gerken, L. & Lucchesi, J
2007Relations between segmental and motor variability in prosodically complex nonword sequences. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 50: 444-458. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gonzalez-Gomez, N. & Nazzi, T
2012Acquisition of nonadjacent phonological dependencies in the native language during the first year of life. Infancy 17: 498-524. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2013Effects of phonotactic prior knowledge on infant word segmentation. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research 56: 840-849. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gonzalez-Gomez, N., Poltrock, S. & Nazzi, T
2013A “Bat” is easier to learn than a “Tab”: Effects of relative phonotactic frequency on infant word learning. PLoS ONE 8: e59601. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grieser, D. & Kuhl, P.K
1989Categorization of speech by infants: Support for speech-sound prototypes. Developmental Psychology 25: 577-588. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grunwell, P
1982Clinical Phonology. London: Croom Helm.Google Scholar
Hallé, P.A., Boysson-Bardies, B. & Vihman, M.M
1991Beginnings of prosodic organization: Intonation and duration patterns of disyllables produced by Japanese and French infants. Language and Speech 34: 299-318.Google Scholar
Höhle, B., Bijeljac-Babic, R., Herold, B., Weissenborn, J. & Nazzi, T
2009The development of language specific prosodic preferences during the first half year of life: Evidence from German and French. Infant Behavior and Development 32: 262-274. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jusczyk, P.W., Cutler, A. & Redanz, N.J
1993Infants’ preference for the predominant stress patterns of English words. Child Development 64: 675–687. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jusczyk, P.W., Houston, D.M. & Newsome, M
1999The beginnings of word segmentation in English-learning infants. Cognitive Psychology 39: 159-207. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Karni, N
2012Minimizing faithfulness violation in the acquisition of Hebrew onset. Brill’s Annual of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics 4: 80-103. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Keren-Portnoy, T., Majorano, M. & Vihman, M.M
2009From phonetics to phonology: The emergence of first words in Italian. Journal of Child Language 36: 235-267. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Khattab, G. & Al-Tamimi, J
2013Influence of geminate structure on early Arabic templatic patterns. In The Emergence of Phonology: Whole Word Approaches, Cross-Linguistic Evidence, M.M. Vihman & T. Keren-Portnoy (eds), 374-414. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kishon-Rabin L., Harel T., Hildesheimer M. & Segal, O
2010Listening preference for the native language compared to an unfamiliar language in hearing and hearing-impaired infants after cochlear implantation. Otology and Neurotology 31: 1275-1280. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kishon-Rabin, L., Taitelbaum, R., Muchnik, C., Gehtler, I., Kronenberg, J. & Hildesheimer, M
2002Development of speech perception and production in children with cochlear implants . The Annals of Otology, Rhinology & Laryngology. Supplement 189: 85-90.Google Scholar
Kuhl, P.K., Conboy, B.T., Coffey-Corina, S., Padden, D., Rivera-Gaxiola, M. & Nelson, T
2008Phonetic learning as a pathway to language: New data and native language magnet theory expanded (NLM-e). Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences 363: 979–1000. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kuhl, P.K., Stevens, E., Hayashi, A., Deguchi, T., Kiritani, S. & Iverson, P
2006Infants show a facilitation effect for native language phonetic perception between 6 and 12 months. Developmental Science 9: 13–21. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kuhl, P.K., Williams, K.A., Lacerda, F., Stevens, K.N. & Lindblom, B
1992Linguistic experience alters phonetic perception in infants by 6 months of age. Science 255: 606-608. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B
2000The CHILDES Project: Tools for analyzing talk, 3rd edn. Mahwah NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Maye, J., Werker, J. & Gerken, L.A
2002Infant sensitivity to distributional information can affect phonetic discrimination. Cognition 8: B101-B111. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Miller, J.L. & Eimas, P.D
1996Internal structure of voicing categories in early infancy. Perception and Psychophysics 58: 1157–1167. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Moon, C., Cooper, R.P. & Fifer, W.P
1993Two-day-olds prefer their native language. Infant Behavior and Development 16: 495-500. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nazzi, T., Bertoncini, J. & Bijeljac-Babic, R
2009A perceptual equivalent of the labial-coronal effect in the first year of life. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 126: 1440–1446. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nazzi, T., Dilley, L.C., Jusczyk, A.M., Shattuck-Hufnagel, S. & Jusczyk, P.W
2005English-learning infants’ segmentation of verbs from fluent speech. Language and Speech 48: 279-298. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nazzi, T., Iakimova, G., Bertoncini, J., Frédonie, S. & Alcantara, C
2006Early segmentation of fluent speech by infants acquiring French: Emerging evidence for cross-linguistic differences. Journal of Memory and Language 54: 283–299. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Oller, D.K., Wieman, L.A., Doyle, W.J. & Ross, C
1976Infant babbling and speech. Journal of Child Language 3: 1-11. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Polka, L., Colantonio, C. & Sundara, M
2001A cross-language comparison of /d/-/ð/ perception: Evidence for a new developmental pattern. Journal of Acoustical Society of America 5: 2190–2200. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Polka, L. & Sundara, M
2012Word segmentation in monolingual infants acquiring Canadian-English and Canadian-French: Native language, cross-language and cross-dialect comparisons. Infancy 17: 198-232. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pons, F. & Bosch, L
2010Stress pattern preference in Spanish-learning infants: The role of syllable weight. Infancy 15: 223-245. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ravid, D
2006Word-level morphology: A psycholinguistic perspective on linear formation in Hebrew nominals. Morphology 16: 127-148. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Saffran, J.R., Aslin, R.N. & Newport, E.L
1996Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science 274: 1926-1928. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Saffran, J.R., Werker, J. & Werner, L
2006The infant's auditory world: Hearing, speech, and the beginnings of language. In Handbook of Child Development, R. Siegler & D. Kuhn (eds), 58-108. New York NY: Wiley.Google Scholar
Savinainen-Makkonen, T
2007Geminate template: A model for first Finnish words. First Language 27: 347-359. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Segal, O., Hejli-Assi, S. & Kishon-Rabin L
2016The effect of listening experience on the discrimination of /ba/ and /pa/ in Hebrew-learning and Arabic-learning infants. Journal of Infant Behavior and Development. 42: 86-99. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Segal, O., Houston, D. & Kishon-Rabin, L
2015Auditory Discrimination of Lexical Stress Patterns in Hearing-Impaired Infants with Cochlear Implants Compared with Normal Hearing: Influence of Acoustic Cues and Listening Experience to the Ambient Language. Ear and hearing 37: 225-234. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Segal, O., Keren-Portnoy, T. & Vihman, M.M
2015Infant Recognition of Hebrew Vocalic Word Patterns. Infancy 20: 208-236. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Segal, O. & Kishon-Rabin, L
2011Listening preference for child-directed speech versus nonspeech stimuli in normal-hearing and hearing-impaired infants after cochlear implantation. Ear and Hearing 32: 358-72. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2012Evidence for language-specific influence on the preference of stress patterns in infants learning an Iambic language (Hebrew). Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 55: 1329-1341. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Segal, O., Nir-Sagiv, B., Kishon-Rabin L. & Ravid, D
2009Prosodic patterns in Hebrew child-directed speech. Journal of Child Language 36: 629-656. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Szreder, M
2013The acquisition of consonant clusters in Polish: A case study. The Emergence of Phonology: Whole Word Approaches, Cross-Linguistic Evidence, M.M. Vihman & T. Keren-Portnoy (eds), 343-361. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tsao, F.-M., Liu, H.-M. & Kuhl, P.K
2004Speech perception in infancy predicts language development in the second year of life: A longitudinal study. Child Development 75: 1067–1084. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tsao, F. -M., Liu, H. -M. & Kuhl, P.K
2006Perception of native and non-native affricate-fricative contrasts: Cross-language tests on adults and infants. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 120: 2285–2294. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Turk, A.E., Jusczyk, P.W. & Gerken, L
1995Do English-learning infants use syllable weight to determine stress? Language and Speech 38: 143-158.Google Scholar
Van Kampen, A., Parmaksiz, G., van de Vijver, R. & Höhle, B
2008Metrical and statistical cues for word segmentation: Vowel harmony and word stress as cues to word boundaries by 6- and 9-month-old Turkish learners. In Language Acquisition and Development, A. Gavarró & M.J. Freitas (eds), 313–324. Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars.Google Scholar
Vihman, M.M
2014Phonological Development: The First Two Years, 2nd edn. Chichester: Wiley Blackwell.Google Scholar
Vihman, M.M. & Croft, W
2007Phonological development: Toward a “radical” templatic phonology. Linguistics 45: 685-725. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vihman, M.M., DePaolis, R.A. & Keren-Portnoy, T
2014The role of production in infant word learning. Language Learning 64 (Supplement 2): 121-140. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vihman, M.M., Kay, E., Boysson-Bardies, B., Durand, C. & Sundberg, U
1994External Sources of Individual Differences? A Cross-Linguistic Analysis of the Phonetics of Mothers' Speech to 1-Year-Old Children, Developmental Psychology 30: 651-662. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vihman, M.M. & Keren-Portnoy, T
(eds) 2013The Emergence of Phonology: Whole Word Approaches, Cross-Linguistic Evidence. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vihman M.M., Macken, M.A., Miller, R., Simmons, H. & Miller, J
1985From babbling to speech: A re-assessment of the continuity issue. Language 61: 397-445. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vouloumanos, A. & Werker, J.F
2004Tuned to the signal: The privileged status of speech for young infants. Developmental Science 7: 270–276. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2007Listening to language at birth: Evidence for a bias for speech in neonates. Developmental Science 10: 159-164. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wauquier, S. & Yamaguchi, N
2013Templates in French. In The Emergence of Phonology: Whole Word Approaches, Cross-Linguistic Evidence, M.M. Vihman & T. Keren-Portnoy (eds), 317-342. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Weber, C., Hahne, A., Friedrich, M. & Friederici, A.D
2004Discrimination of word stress in early infant perception: Electrophysiological evidence. Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research 18: 149-161. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Werker, J.F., Gilbert, J.V.H., Humphrey, K. & Tees, R.C
1981Developmental aspects of cross language speech perception. Child Development 52: 349-355. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Werker, J.F. & Lalonde, C
1988Cross-language speech perception: Initial capabilities and developmental change. Developmental Psychology 24: 672–683. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Werker, J.F. & Mcleod, P.J
1989Infant preference to both male and female infant-directed talk: A developmental study of attentional effective responsiveness. Canadian Journal of Psychology 43: 230-246. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Werker, J.F. & Tees, R.C
1983Developmental changes across childhood in the perception of nonnative sounds. Canadian Journal of Psychology 37: 27S-286. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
1984Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behavior and Development 7: 49-63. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
1999Influences on infant speech processing: Toward a new synthesis. Annual Review of Psychology 50: 509-535. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Whalen, D.H., Levitt, A.G. & Wang, Q
1991Intonational differences between the reduplicative babbling of French- and English-learning infants. Journal of Child Language 18: 501-516. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 6 other publications

Aalto, Eija, Katri Saaristo-Helin & Suvi Stolt
2023. Auditory Word Recognition Ability in Babble Noise and Phonological Development in Children at 3;6 Years of Age. Language Learning and Development 19:2  pp. 230 ff. DOI logo
Segal, Osnat, Tamar Keren‐Portnoy & Marilyn Vihman
2020. Robust effects of stress on early lexical representation. Infancy 25:4  pp. 500 ff. DOI logo
Vihman, Marilyn
2018. Chapter 10. The development of prosodic structure. In The Development of Prosody in First Language Acquisition [Trends in Language Acquisition Research, 23],  pp. 185 ff. DOI logo
Vihman, Marilyn & Sophie Wauquier
2018. Chapter 1. Templates in child language. In Sources of Variation in First Language Acquisition [Trends in Language Acquisition Research, 22],  pp. 27 ff. DOI logo
Vihman, Marilyn M.
2022. How to learn more about how children learn languages: A commentary on Kidd and García (2022). First Language 42:6  pp. 827 ff. DOI logo
Vihman, Marilyn May, Mitsuhiko Ota, Tamar Keren-Portnoy, Rui Qi Choo & Shanshan Lou
2023. A Challenge to Whole-word Phonology? A Study of Japanese and Mandarin. Language Learning and Development 19:4  pp. 480 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 21 march 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.