References
Bergelson, E. & Swingley, D.
2012At 6–9 months, human infants know the meanings of many common nouns. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 109(9): 3253–3258. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2013The acquisition of abstract words by young infants. Cognition 127(3): 391–397. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bernal, S., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Millotte, S. & Christophe, A.
2010Two-year-olds compute syntactic structure online. Developmental Science 12: 69–76. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bernal, S., Lidz, J., Millotte, S. & Christophe, A.
2007Syntax constrains the acquisition of verb meaning. Language Learning and Development 3: 325–341. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brusini, P., Amsili, P., Chemla, E. & Christophe, A.
2011Learning to categorize nouns and verbs on the basis of a few known examples: A computational model relying on 2-word contexts. 2011 Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research on Child Development, Montreal, Canada.Google Scholar
2014Simulation de l’apprentissage des contextes nominaux/verbaux par n-gramme. Paper presented at the Traitement Automatique des Langues Naturelles, Marseille, France.
Brusini, P., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Dutat, M., Goffinet, F. & Christophe, A.
2016ERP evidence for online syntactic computations in 2-year-olds. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 19: 164–173. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brusini, P., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Van Heugten, M., de Carvalho, A., Goffinet, F., Fiévet, A-C. & Christophe, A.
2016Ambiguous function words do not prevent 18-month-olds from building accurate syntactic category expectations: An ERP study. Neuropsychologia. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carey, S.
2009The Origin of Concepts. Oxford: OUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cauvet, E., Limissuri, R., Millotte, S., Skoruppa, K., Cabrol, D. & Christophe, A.
2014Function words constrain online recognition of verbs and nouns in French 18-month-olds. Language Learning & Development 10: 1–18. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chemla, E., Mintz, T. H., Bernal, S. & Christophe, A.
2009Categorizing words using “Frequent Frames”: What cross-linguistic analyses reveal about distributional acquisition strategies. Developmental Science 12: 396–406. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Christophe, A., Millotte, S., Bernal, S. & Lidz, J.
2008Bootstrapping lexical and syntactic acquisition. Language and Speech 51: 61–75. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Christophe, A., Nespor, M., Guasti, M. T. & van Ooyen, B.
2003Prosodic structure and syntactic acquisition: The case of the head-direction parameter. Developmental Science 6: 211–220. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Conwell, E. & Morgan, J.
2012Is it a Noun or is it a Verb? Resolving the ambicategoriality problem in language acquisition. Language Learning and Development 8: 87–112. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dautriche, I., Fibla, L. & Christophe, A.
2015Learning homophones: Syntactic and semantic context matters. Presented at the 40th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, Boston MA.Google Scholar
de Carvalho, A., Dautriche, I. & Christophe, A.
2014Phrasal prosody constrains online syntactic analysis in two-year-old children. Presented at the 39th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, Boston MA.Google Scholar
2016Preschoolers use phrasal prosody online to constrain syntactic analysis. Developmental Science 19: 235–250. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
de Carvalho, A., Dautriche, I., Lin, I. & Christophe, A.
2017Phrasal prosody constrains syntactic analysis in toddlers. Cognition, 163, 67–79. DOI logo.Google Scholar
de Carvalho, A., He, A. X., Lidz, J. & Christophe, A.
201518-month-olds use phrasal prosody as a cue to constrain the acquisition of novel word meanings. Presented at the 40th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, Boston MA.Google Scholar
Ferry, A., Flo, A., Brusini, P., Cattarossi, L., Macagno, F., Nespor, M. & Mehler, J.
2016On the edge of language acquisition: Inherent constraints on encoding multisyllabic sequences in the neonate brain. Developmental Science 19: 488–503. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gerken, L., Jusczyk, P. W. & Mandel, D. R.
1994When prosody fails to cue syntactic structure: 9-month-olds' sensitivity to phonological versus syntactic phrases. Cognition 51: 237–265. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gleitman, L.
1990The structural sources of verb meanings. Language Acquisition 1: 3–55. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gout, A., Christophe, A. & Morgan, J.
2004Phonological phrase boundaries constrain lexical access: II. Infant data. Journal of Memory and Language 51: 548–567. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gutman, A., Dautriche, I., Crabbé, B. & Christophe, A.
2015Bootstrapping the syntactic bootstrapper: Probabilistic labelling of prosodic phrases. Language Acquisition 22: 285–309. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hallé, P., Durand, C. & de Boysson-Bardies, B.
2008Do 11-month-old French infants process articles? Language and Speech 51: 45–66. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hawthorne, K. & Gerken, L.
2014From pauses to clauses: Prosody facilitates learning of syntactic constituency. Cognition 133(2): 420–428. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hawthorne, K., Mazuka, R. & Gerken, L.
2015The acoustic salience of prosody trumps infants' acquired knowledge of language-specific prosodic patterns. Journal of Memory and Language 82: 105–117. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
He, A. X. & Lidz, J.
2014Development of the verb-event link between 14 and 18 months. Paper presented at the 39th Boston University Conference on Language Development, Boston MA.
Höhle, B., Weissenborn, J., Kiefer, D., Schulz, A. & Schmitz, M.
2004Functional elements in infants' speech processing: The role of determiners in the syntactic categorization of lexical elements. Infancy 5: 341–353. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jakubowicz, C., Nash, L., Rigaut, C. & Gérard, C. -L.
1998Determiners and clitic pronouns in French-speaking children with SLI. Language Acquisition 7: 113–160. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Johnson, E. K., Seidl, A. & Tyler, M. D.
2014The edge factor in early word segmentation: Utterance-level prosody enables word form extraction by 6-month-olds. Plos One 9(1): e83546. 1–14. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Johnson, M.
2013Language acquisition as statistical inference. In The Language-Cognition Interface, S. R. Anderson, J. Moeschler & F. Reboul (eds), 109–134. Geneva: Libraire Droz.Google Scholar
Kedar, Y., Casasola, M. & Lust, B.
2006Getting there faster: 18- and 24-month-old infants’ use of function words to determine reference. Child Development 77: 325–338. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kjelgaard, M. M. & Speer, S. R.
1999Prosodic facilitation and interference in the resolution of temporary syntactic closure ambiguity. Journal of Memory and Language 40: 153–194. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mehler, J., Jusczyk, P. W., Lambertz, G., Halsted, G., Bertoncini, J. & Amiel-Tison, C.
1988A precursor of language acquisition in young infants. Cognition 29: 143–178. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Millotte, S., Morgan, J., Margules, S., Bernal, S., Dutat, M. & Christophe, A.
2010Phrasal prosody constrains word segmentation in French 16-month-olds. Journal of Portuguese Linguistics 9: 67–86.Google Scholar
Millotte, S., René, A., Wales, R. & Christophe, A.
2008Phonological phrase boundaries constrain online syntactic analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 34: 874–885.Google Scholar
Millotte, S., Wales, R. & Christophe, A.
2007Phrasal prosody disambiguates syntax. Language and Cognitive Processes 22: 898–909. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Millotte, S., Wales, R., Dupoux, E. & Christophe, A.
2006Can prosodic cues and function words guide syntactic processing and acquisition? In Speech prosody: 3rd International Conference, R. Hoffmann & H. Mixdorff (eds), 174–177. Dresden: Dresden TUD Press.Google Scholar
Mintz, T. H.
2003Frequent frames as a cue for grammatical categories in child-directed speech. Cognition 90: 91–117. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morgan, J. L.
1986From Simple Input to Complex Grammar. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Morgan, J. L. & Demuth, K.
1996Signal to syntax: An overview. In Signal to Syntax: Bootstrapping from Speech to Grammar in Early Acquisition, J. L. Morgan & K. Demuth (eds), 1–22. Mahwah NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Nespor, M. & Vogel, I.
1986Prosodic Phonology. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar
Saxe, R. & Carey, S.
2006The perception of causality in infancy. Acta Psychologica 123(1–2): 144–165. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Selkirk, E. O.
1984Phonology and Syntax: The Relation between Sound and Structure. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Shattuck-Hufnagel, S. & Turk, A. E.
1996A prosody tutorial for investigators of auditory sentence processing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 25: 193–247. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shi, R.
2014Functional morphemes and early language acquisition. Child Development Perspectives 8(1): 6–11. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shi, R., Cutler, A., Werker, J. & Cruickshank, M.
2006Frequency and form as determinants of functor sensitivity in English-acquiring infants. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 119: EL61–EL66. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shi, R. & Gauthier, B.
2005Recognition of function words in 8-month-old French-learning infants. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 117: 2426–2427. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shi, R. & Melançon, A.
2010Syntactic categorization in French-learning infants. Infancy 15: 517–533. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shi, R., Morgan, J. L. & Allopenna, P.
1998Phonological and acoustic bases for earliest grammatical category assignment: A cross-linguistic perspective. Journal of Child Language 25: 169–201. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shukla, M., Nespor, M. & Mehler, J.
2007An interaction between prosody and statistics in the segmentation of fluent speech. Cognitive Psychology 54: 1–32. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Snedeker, J. & Yuan, S.
2008Effects of prosodic and lexical constraints on parsing in young children (and adults). Journal of Memory and Language 58(2): 574–608. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Heugten, M. & Johnson, E. K.
2011Gender-marked determiners help Dutch learners' word recognition when gender information itself does not. Journal of Child Language 38(1): 87–100. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Veneziano, E. & Parisse, C.
2011Retrieving the meaning of words from syntactic cues: A comprehension study. Presented at the XIIIth International Conference on the Study of Child Language, Montreal, Canada.Google Scholar
Veneziano, E. & Sinclair, H.
2000The changing status of “filler syllables” on the way to grammatical morphemes. Journal of Child Language 27: 1–40. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Waxman, S. R., Lidz, J. L., Braun, I. E. & Lavin, T.
2009Twenty four-month-old infants' interpretations of novel verbs and nouns in dynamic scenes. Cognitive Psychology 59(1): 67–95. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zangl, R. & Fernald, A.
2007Increasing flexibility in children’s online processing of grammatical and nonce determiners in fluent speech. Language Learning and Development 3: 199–231. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 2 other publications

Benítez‐Burraco, Antonio & Wendy Elvira‐García
2023. Human self‐domestication and the evolution of prosody. Language and Linguistics Compass 17:3 DOI logo
Vapnarsky, Valentina & Edy Veneziano

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 22 march 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.