Chapter 8
Conversational partners and common ground
Variation contributes to language acquisition
Eve V. Clark |
Department of Linguistics, Stanford University
Children’s early exposure to and experience with language varies. They differ in how much conversational interaction they have with more expert adult speakers, which affects both speed of processing and early vocabulary acquisition. Another source of variation is how many interlocutors children interact with. Interlocutors are male or female, use different dialects, and know differing amounts about the child’s daily routines. Here, how much practice children have with interlocutors inside and outside the family affects how well children can establish and make use of common ground. Children also differ in how much practice they have, early on, in making themselves understood to others, and this affects how readily they can establish and then add to common ground with each new interlocutor.
Article outline
- How much interactive language are children exposed to early on?
- Consequences of differences in amount of interaction
- Common ground
- Conversational partners
- Common ground for adult and child
- Adding conversational partners
- How do speakers establish a starting point?
- How do speakers add new information to existing common ground?
- Assessing what the other knows
- Linguistic devices for signaling given and new
- Conclusions
-
Acknowledgements
-
References
References
References
Bloom, L., Margulis, C., Tinker, E. & Fujita, N.
1996 Early conversation and word learning: Contributions from child and adult.
Child Development 67: 3154–3175.


Bresson, F.
1977 Syntax, semantics, and utterance: Determining a referent.
International Journal of Psycholinguistics 4: 31–41.

Brown, R. W.
1973 A First Language: The Early Stages. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.


Bruner, J. S.
1983 Child Talk. London: Fontana.

Chouinard, M. M. & Clark, E. V.
2003 Adult reformulations of child errors as negative evidence.
Journal of Child Language 30: 637–669.


Clark, E. V.
1997 Conceptual perspective and lexical choice in acquisition.
Cognition 64: 1–37.


Clark, E. V.
2007 Young children’s uptake of new words in conversation.
Language in Society 36: 157–182.


Clark, E. V.
2010 Adult offer, word-class, and child uptake in early lexical acquisition.
First Language 30: 250–269.


Clark, E. V.
2015 Common ground. In
The Handbook of Language Emergence,
B. MacWhinney &
W. O’Grady (eds), 328–353. London: Wiley-Blackwell.

Clark, E. V. & Bernicot, J.
2008 Repetition as ratification: How parents and children place information in common ground.
Journal of Child Language 35: 349–371.


Clark, E. V. & Chouinard, M. M.
2000 Enoncés enfantins et reformulations adultes dans l’acquisition du langage.
Langages 140: 9–23.


Clark, H. H.
1996 Using Language. Cambridge: CUP.


Corrin, J.
2010 Maternal repair initiation at MLU Stage I: The developmental power of ‘hm?’ First Language 30: 312–328.


Corsaro, W. A.
1977 The clarification request as a feature of adult interactive styles with young children.
Language in Society 6: 183–207.


Estigarribia, B.
2010 Facilitation by variation: right-to-left learning of English yes/no questions.
Cognitive Science 34(1), 68–93.

Fernald, A. & Marchman, V. A.
2012 Individual differences in lexical processing at 18 months predict vocabulary growth in typically developing and late-talking toddlers.
Child Development 83: 203–222.


Fernald, A., Perfors, A. & Marchman, V. A.
2006 Picking up speed in understanding: Speech processing efficiency and vocabulary growth across the second year.
Development Psychology 42: 98–116.


Fisher, C. & Tokura, H.
1995 The given-new contract in speech to infants.
Journal of Memory and Language 34: 287–310.


Floccia, C., Delle Luche, C., Durrant, S., Butler, J. & Goslin, J.
2012 Parents or community: Where do 20-month-olds exposed to two accents acquire their representations of words? Cognition 124: 95–100.


Hart, B. & Risley, T. R.
1995 Meaningful Differences in the Everyday Experience of Young American Children. Baltimore MD: Paul H. Brookes.

Haviland, S. E. & Clark, H. H.
1974 What’s new? Acquiring new information as a process in comprehension.
Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 13: 512–521.


Hoff, E.
2006 How social contexts support and shape language development.
Developmental Review 26: 55–88.


Käsermann, M. -L. & Foppa, K.
1981 Some determinants of self-correction: An interactional study of Swiss German. In
The Child’s Construction of Language,
W. Deutsch (ed.), 77–104. London: Academic Press.

Liebal, K., Behne, T., Carpenter, M. & Tomasello, M.
2009 Infants use shared experience to interpret pointing gestures.
Developmental Science 12: 264–271.


Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M. & Tomasello, M.
2008 Twelve-month-olds communicate helpfully and appropriately for knowledgeable and ignorant partners.
Cognition 108: 732–739.


Maratsos, M. P.
1974 Preschool children’s use of definite and indefinite articles.
Child Development 45: 446–455.


Marcos, H., Salazar Orvig, A., Bernicot, J., Guidetti, M., Hudelot, C. & Préneron, C.
2004 Apprendre à Parler: Influence du Mode de Garde. Paris: L’Harmattan.

O’Neill, D. K.
1996 Two-year-olds’ sensitivity to the parent’s knowledge when making requests.
Child Development 67: 659–677.


Place, S. & Hoff, E.
2011 Properties of dual language exposure that influence 2-year-olds’ bilingual proficiency.
Child Development 82: 1834–1849.


Scollon, R.
1976 Conversations with a One-year-old. Honolulu HI: University of Hawaii Press.

Seidl, A., Onishi, K. H. & Cristia, A.
2014 Talker variation aids young infants’ phonotactic learning.
Language Learning and Development 10: 297–307.


Snow, C. E.
1978 The conversational context of language acquisition. In
Recent Advances in the Psychology of Language,
R. N. Campbell &
P. T. Smith (eds), 253–269. London: Plenum.

Tarplee, C.
2011 Next turn and intersubjectivity in children’s language acquisition. In
Analysing Interactions in Children,
H. Gardner &
M. Forrrester (eds), 3–22. London: Wiley-Blackwell.

Tomasello, M., Farrar, M. J. & Dines, J.
1984 Children’s speech revisions for a familiar and an unfamiliar adult.
Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 27: 359–363.


Veneziano, E.
1988 Vocal-verbal interaction and the construction of early lexical knowledge. In
The Emergent Lexicon: The Child’s Development of a Linguistic Vocabulary,
M. D. Smith & &
J. L. Locke (eds), 109–147. New York NY: Academic Press.

Warden, D. A.
1976 The influence of context on children’s use of identifying expressions and references.
British Journal of Psychology 67: 101–112.


Weisleder, A. & Fernald, A.
2013 Talking to children matters: Early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary.
Psychological Science 24: 2143–2152.


Zimmerman, F. J., Gilkerson, J., Richards, J. A., Christakis, D. A., Xu, D., Gray, S. & Yapanel, U.
2009 Teaching by listening: The importance of adult-child conversations to language development.
Pediatrics 124: 342–349.


Cited by
Cited by 3 other publications
Bassano, Dominique, Florence Labrell & Philippe Bonnet
2020.
Le Développement du langage de production en français (DLPF) entre 18 et 42 mois : une synthèse.
Enfance N° 2:2
► pp. 151 ff.

Bryant, Judith Becker
2023.
The promise and practice of teaching social communication skills: A commentary on Abbot-Smith et al., 2023.
First Language 
Sudartinah, Titik
2023.
A Conversation Analysis of Two-year-old Children’s Speech in Child-Adult Interactions.
Jurnal Obsesi : Jurnal Pendidikan Anak Usia Dini 7:3
► pp. 3345 ff.

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 18 november 2023. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.