Part of
The Development of Prosody in First Language Acquisition
Edited by Pilar Prieto and Núria Esteve-Gibert
[Trends in Language Acquisition Research 23] 2018
► pp. 125143
References (76)
References
Abercrombie, D. (1967). Elements of general phonetics. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Allen G. D. (1983). Some suprasegmental contours in French two-year-old children’s speech. Phonetica, 40, 269–292. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Allen, G. D., & Hawkins, S. (1978). The development of phonological rhythm. In A. Bell, & J. Hooper (Eds.), Syllables and segments (pp.172–185). Amsterdam: North-Holland Publishing.Google Scholar
(1980). Phonological rhythm: Definition and development. In G. H. Yeni-Komshian, J. F. Kavanagh, & C. A. Ferguson (Eds.), Child phonology. Volume 1: Production (pp.227–256). New York, NY: Academic Press.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Armstrong, L. (1932). The phonetics of French. London: G. Bell and Sons.Google Scholar
Arvaniti, A. (2012). The usefulness of metrics in the quantification of speech rhythm. Journal of Phonetics, 40, 351–373. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Astruc, L., Payne, E., Post, B., Vanrell, M. M., & Prieto, P. (2013). Tonal targets in early child Catalan, Spanish, and English. Language and Speech, 56(2), 229–253. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bertinetto, P. M. (1981). Strutture prosodiche dell’italiano. Firenze: Accademia della Crusca.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Luk, G., Peets, K. F., & Yang, S. (2010). Receptive vocabulary differences in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13(4), 525–531. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bunta, F., & Ingram, D. (2007). The acquisition of speech rhythm by bilingual Spanish- and English-speaking 4- and 5-year-old children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50(4), 999–1014. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chen, A., & Fikkert, P. (2007 ). Intonation of early two-word utterances in Dutch. Proceedings of the 16th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences – Saarbrücken, 315–320.Google Scholar
Cummins, F. (2015). Rhythm and speech. In M. A. Redford (Ed.), The handbook of speech production (pp.158–177). Chichester: John Wiley and Sons.Google Scholar
Cutler, A. (1976). Phoneme-monitoring reaction time as a function of preceding intonation contour. Perception and Psychophysics, 20, 55–60. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1991). Linguistic rhythm and speech segmentation. In J. Sundberg, L. Nord, & R. Carlson (Eds.), Music, language, speech and brain (pp.157–166). London: Macmillan. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cutler, A., & Foss, D. J. (1977). On the role of sentence stress in sentence processing. Language and Speech, 21, 1–10 DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dasher, R., & Bolinger, D. (1982). On pre-accentual lengthening. Journal of the International Phonetic Association, 12, 58–71. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dauer, R. M. (1983). Stress-timing and syllable-timing reanalyzed. Journal of Phonetics, 11, 51–62.Google Scholar
(1987). Phonetic and phonological components of language rhythm. Proceedings of the 11th ICPhS – Tallinn, 447–450.Google Scholar
Davis, B. L., MacNeilage, P. F., & Matyear, C. L. (2002). Acquisition of serial complexity in speech production: A comparison of phonetic and phonological approaches to first word production. Phonetica, 59, 75–109. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Delattre, P. (1966). A comparison of syllable length conditioning among languages. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 4, 183–198. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1969). An acoustic and articulatory study of vowel reduction in four languages. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 7, 295–325. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
DePaolis, R. A., Vihman, M. M., & Kunnari, S. (2008). Prosody in production at the onset of word use: A cross-linguistic study. Journal of Phonetics, 36, 406–426. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Echols, C., Crowhurst, M., & Childers, C. (1997). Perception of rhythmic units in speech by infants and adults, Journal of Memory and Language, 36, 202–225. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grabe, E., Gut, U., Post, B., & Watson, I. (2001). The acquisition of rhythm in English, French and German. Proceedings of the Child Language Seminar 1999 – London, 156–162.Google Scholar
Grabe, E., & Low, E. L. (2002). Durational variability in speech and the rhythm class hypothesis. In C. Gussenhoven, & N. Warner (Eds.), Papers in Laboratory Phonology 7 (pp.515–546). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Grabe, E., Watson, I., & Post, B. (1999). The acquisition of rhythmic patterns in English and French. Proceedings of the 14th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences – San Francisco, 1201–1204.Google Scholar
Hallé, P., de Boysson-Bardies, B., & Vihman, M. (1991). Beginnings of prosodic organisation: Intonation and duration patterns of disyllables produced by Japanese and French infants. Language and Speech, 34(4), 299–318.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hawkins, S. (2014). Situational influences on rhythmicity in speech, music, and their interaction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, 369, 20130398. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hawkins, S., Cross I., & Ogden, R. (2013). Communicative interaction in spontaneous music and speech. In M. Orwin, C. Howes, & R. Kempson (Eds.), Language, music and interaction, (pp. 285–329). London: College Publications.Google Scholar
Huggins, A. W. F. (1972). On the perception of temporal phenomena in speech. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 51, 1279–1290. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Johnson, E., & Jusczyk, P. (2001). Word segmentation by 8-month-olds: When speech cues count for more than statistics. Journal of Memory and Language, 44, 548–567. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jusczyk, P. (1997). The discovery of spoken language. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
(1999). How infants begin to extract words from fluent speech. Trends in Cognitive Science, 3, 323–328. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jusczyk, P., Houston, D., & Newsome, M. (1999). The beginnings of word segmentation in English-learning infants. Cognitive Psychology, 39, 159–207. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kehoe, M., Lleó, C., & Rakow, M. (2011). Speech rhythm in the pronunciation of German and Spanish monolingual and German-Spanish bilingual 3-year-olds. Linguistische Berichte, 227, 323–351.Google Scholar
Konopczynski, G. (1995). A developmental model of acquisition of rhythmic patterns: Results from a cross-linguistic study. Proceedings of the Intonational Congress of Phonetic Sciences – Stockholm, Vol. 4, 22–25.Google Scholar
Ladefoged, P. (1975). A course in phonetics (1st. ed.). New York, NY: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.Google Scholar
Li, A., & Post, B. (2014). L2 acquisition of prosodic properties of speech rhythm: Evidence from L1 Mandarin and German learners of English. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 36(2), 223–255. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lleó, C., Rakow, M., & Kehoe, M. (2007). Acquiring rhythmically different languages in a bilingual context. Proceedings of the 16th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences – Saarbrücken, 1545–1548.Google Scholar
Lloyd James, A. (1940). Speech signals in telephony. London: Pitman and Son.Google Scholar
Low, E. L., Grabe, E., & Nolan F. (2000). Quantitative characterisations of speech rhythm: “Syllable-timing” in Singapore English. Language and Speech, 43(1), 377–401.Google Scholar
Mok, P. (2011). The acquisition of speech rhythm by three-year-old bilingual and monolingual children: Cantonese and English. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14(4), 458–472. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2013). Speech rhythm of monolingual and bilingual children at 2;06: Cantonese and English. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16, 693–703. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morgan. J. L. (1986). From simple input to complex grammar. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Nazzi, T., Bertoncini, J., & Mehler, J. (1998). Language discrimination by newborns: Towards an understanding of the role of rhythm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 756–766.Google Scholar
Nazzi, T., Jusczyk, P. W., & Johnson, E. K. (2000). Language discrimination by English learning 5-month-olds: Effects of rhythm and familiarity. Journal of Memory and Language, 43, 1–19. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nazzi, T., & Ramus, F. (2003). Perception and acquisition of linguistic rhythm by infants. Speech Communication, 41, 233–243. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nolan, F., & Jeon, H.-S. (2014). Speech rhythm: A metaphor? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 369, 20130396. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ordin, M., & Polyanskaya, L. (2014). Development of timing patterns in first and second languages. System, 42, 244–257. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J. (2001). Do bilingual two-year-olds have separate phonological systems. International Journal of Bilingualism, 5(1), 19–38. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Payne, E., Post, B., Astruc, L., Prieto, P., & Vanrell, M. M. (2010). Rhythmic modification in child directed speech. In M. Russo (Ed.), Prosodic Universals. Comparative Studies in Rhythmic Modeling and Rhythm Typology (pp.147–184). Roma: Aracne Editrice.Google Scholar
Payne, E., Post, B., Prieto, P., Vanrell, M. M., & Astruc, L. (2012). Measuring child rhythm. Language and Speech, 55, 202–228. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Phillips-Silver, J., & Trainor, L. J. (2005). Feeling the beat: Movement influences infant rhythm perception. Science, 308, 1483. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pike, J. L. (1945). The intonation of American English. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
Prieto, P., & Vanrell, M. M. (2009). L’adquisició de l’entonació del català. In Devís, E., & Carol, L. (Eds.), Studi catalani. Suoni e parole (pp.67–82). Bologna: Bononia University Press.Google Scholar
Prieto, P., Vanrell, M. M., Astruc, L., Payne, E., & Post, B. (2012). Phonotactic and phrasal properties of speech rhythm. Evidence from Catalan, English, and Spanish. Speech Communication, 54(6), 681–702. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ramus, F. (2002). Language discrimination by newborns: Teasing apart phonotactic, rhythmic, and intonational cues. Annual Review of Language Acquisition, 2, 85–115. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ramus, F., Dupoux, E., & Mehler, J. (2003). The psychological reality of rhythm classes: Perceptual studies. Proceedings of the 15th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences – Barcelona, 337–342.Google Scholar
Ramus, F., Hauser, M., Miller, C., Morris, D., & Mehler, J. (2000). Language discrimination by human newborns and by cotton-top tamarin monkeys. Science, 288(5464), 349–351. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ramus, F., & Mehler, J. (1999). Language identification with suprasegmental cues: A study based on speech resynthesis, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 105(1), 512–521. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ramus, F., & Nespor, M., & Mehler, J. (1999). Correlates of linguistic rhythm in the speech signal, Cognition, 73, 265–292. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rathcke, T., & Smith, R. (2015). Speech timing and linguistic rhythm: On the acoustic bases of rhythm typologies. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 137, 2834–2845. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roach, P. (1982). On the distinction between ‘stress-timed’ and ‘syllable-timed’ languages. In D. Crystal (Ed.), Linguistic controversies (pp.73–79). London: Edward Arnold.Google Scholar
Schmidt, A-E., & Post, B. (2015a). Language interaction in the development of speech rhythm in simultaneous bilinguals. In E. Delais-Roussarie, M. Avanzi, & S. Herment (Eds.), Prosody and language in contact (pp.271–291). Heidelberg: Springer.Google Scholar
(2015b). The development of prosodic features and their contribution to rhythm production in simultaneous bilinguals. Language and Speech, 58(1), 24–47. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shields, J. L., McHugh, A., & Martin, J. G. (1974). Reaction time to phoneme targets as a function of rhythmic cues in continuous speech. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 102(2), 250–255. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Snow, D. (1994). Phrase-final syllable lengthening and intonation in early child speech. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37, 831–840. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vihman, M. M. (2014). Phonological development: The origins of language in the child. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Vihman, M. M., Davis, B. L., & DePaolis, R. A. (1995). Prosodic analysis of babbling and first words: A comparison of English and French. Proceedings of the XIIIth International Congress of Phonetic Sciences – Stockholm, Vol. 4, 14–27.Google Scholar
Vihman, M. M., Nakai, S., & DePaolis, R. A. (2006). Getting the rhythm right: A cross-linguistic study of segmental duration in babbling and first words. In L. Goldstein, D. Whalen, & C. Best (Eds.), Laboratory phonology 8: Phonology and phonetics (pp.341–366). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Whalen, D. H., Levitt, A. G., & Wang, Q. (1991). Intonational differences between the reduplicative babbling of French-and English-learning infants. Journal of Child Language, 18(3), 501–516. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
White, L. (2014). Communicative function and prosodic form in speech timing. Speech Communication, 63–64, 38–54. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
White, L., Delle Luche, C., & Floccia, C. (2016). Five-month-old infants’ discrimination of unfamiliar languages does not accord with “rhythm class”. Proceedings of Speech Prosody 2016 – Boston, 567–571.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
White, L., Floccia, C., Goslin, J., & Butler, J. (2014). Utterance-final lengthening is predictive of infants’ discrimination of English accents. Language Learning, 64, 27–44. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
White, L., Mattys, S. L., & Wiget, L. (2012). Language categorization by adults is based on sensitivity to durational cues, not rhythm class. Journal of Memory and Language, 66, 665–679. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Whitworth, N. (2002). Speech rhythm production in three German-English bilingual families. Leeds Working Papers in Linguistics and Phonetics, 9, 175–205.Google Scholar
Cited by (4)

Cited by four other publications

Fuchs, Robert
2023. Rhythm Metrics and the Perception of Rhythmicity in Varieties of English as a Second Language. In Speech Rhythm in Learner and Second Language Varieties of English [Prosody, Phonology and Phonetics, ],  pp. 187 ff. DOI logo
Maxwell, Olga & Elinor Payne
2023. Investigating (Rhythm) Variation in Indian English: An Integrated Approach. In Speech Rhythm in Learner and Second Language Varieties of English [Prosody, Phonology and Phonetics, ],  pp. 17 ff. DOI logo
VIHMAN, Marilyn May, Mitsuhiko OTA, Tamar KEREN-PORTNOY, Shanshan LOU & Rui Qi CHOO
2023. Child phonological responses to variegation in adult words: A cross-linguistic study. Journal of Child Language 50:6  pp. 1459 ff. DOI logo
Kent, Ray D. & Carrie Rountrey
2020. What Acoustic Studies Tell Us About Vowels in Developing and Disordered Speech. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 29:3  pp. 1749 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 28 september 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.