Chapter published in:
The Development of Prosody in First Language Acquisition
Edited by Pilar Prieto and Núria Esteve-Gibert
[Trends in Language Acquisition Research 23] 2018
► pp. 227246
References

References

Armstrong, M. E.
(2012) The development of yes-no question intonation in Puerto Rican Spanish. Unpublished PhD dissertation. Ohio State University.
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U.
(1985) Does the autistic child have a “Theory of Mind”? Cognition, 21, 37–46. Crossref logo
Bates, E., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V.
(1975) The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly of Behavior and Development, 21(3), 205–226.
Beaumont, S. L., & Bloom, K.
(1993) Adults’ attributions of intentionality to vocalizing infants. First Language, 13, 235–247. Crossref logo
Berman, J. M. J., Chambers, C. G., & Graham, S. A.
(2016) Preschoolers’ real-time coordination of vocal and facial emotional information. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 142, 391–399. Crossref logo
Blasi, A., Mercure, E., Lloyd-Fox, S., Thomson, A., Brammer, M., Sauter, D., Deeley, Q., Barker, G. J., Renvall, V., Deoni, S., Gasston, D., Williams, S. C. R., Johnson, M. H., Simmons, A., & Murphy, D. G. M.
(2011) Early specialization for voice and emotion processing in the infant brain. Current Biology, 21(14), 1220–1224. Crossref logo
Bruner, J. S.
(1975) The ontogeny of speech acts. Journal of Child Language, 2(1), 1–19. Crossref logo
Cameron-Faulkner, T.
(2014) The development of speech acts. In D. Matthews (Ed.), Pragmatic development in first language acquisition (pp.37–52). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Carpenter, M., Akhtar, N., & Tomasello, M.
(1998) Fourteen- through 18-month-old infants differentially imitate intentional and accidental actions. Infant Behavior & Development, 21(2), 315–330. Crossref logo
Clark, E. V.
(2004) Pragmatics and language acquisition. In L. Horn, & G. Ward (Eds.), The handbook of pragmatics (pp.562–577). Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Csibra, G., & Gergely, G.
(2009) Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(4), 148–153. Crossref logo
Davis, B. L., MacNeilage, P. F., Matyear, C. L., & Powell, J. K.
(2000) Prosodic correlates of stress in babbling: An acoustical study. Child Development, 71(5), 1258–1270. Crossref logo
DeCasper, A. J., & Fifer, W. P.
(1980) Of human bonding: Newborns prefer their mothers voices. Science, 208,1174–1176. Crossref logo
De Villiers, J.
(2007) The interface of language and Theory of Mind. Lingua. International Review of General Linguistics. Revue Internationale de Linguistique Générale, 117(11), 1858–1878.
Esteve-Gibert, N., Liszkowski, U., & Prieto, P.
(2016) Prosodic and gestural features distinguish the intention of pointing gestures in child-directed communication. In M. E. Armstrong, N. Henriksen, & M. M. Vanrell (Eds.), Intonational grammar in Ibero-Romance. Approaches across linguistic subfields (pp.249–276). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Esteve-Gibert, N., & Prieto P.
(2013) Prosody signals the emergence of intentional communication in the first year of life: Evidence from Catalan-babbling infants. Journal of Child Language, 40(5), 919–944. Crossref logo
Esteve-Gibert, N., Prieto, P., & Liszkowski, U.
(2017) Twelve-month-olds understand social intentions based on prosody and gesture shape. Infancy, 22(1), 108–129. Doi:Crossref logo.
Fernald, A.
(1993) Approval and disapproval: Infant responsiveness to vocal affect in familiar and unfamiliar languages. Child Development, 64, 657–674. Crossref logo
(1985) Four-month-old infants prefer to listen to motherese. Infant Behavior and Development, 8(2), 181–195. Crossref logo
Frota, S., Butler, J., & Vigário, M.
(2014) Infants perception of intonation: Is it a statement or a question? Infancy, 19(2), 194–213. Crossref logo
Goldin-Meadow, S., & Alibali, M. W.
(2013) Gestures role in speaking, learning, and creating language. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 257–283. Crossref logo
Goldstein, M. H., Schwade, J. A., & Bornstein, M. H.
(2009) The value of vocalizing: Five-month-old infants associate their own noncry vocalizations with responses from caregivers. Child Development, 80(3), 636–644. Crossref logo
Grossmann, T., Oberecker, R., Koch, S. P., & Friederici, A. D.
(2010) The developmental origins of voice processing in the human brain. Neuron, 65(6), 852–858. Crossref logo
Grünloh, T., & Liszkowski, U.
(2015Prelinguistic vocalizations distinguish pointing acts. Journal of Child Language, 42(6), 1312–1336. Crossref logo
Gussenhoven, C.
(1984) On the grammar and semantics of sentence accents. Dordrecht: Foris.Crossref logo
Höhle, B., Bijeljac-Babic, R., Herold, B., Weissenborn, J., & Nazzi, T.
(2009) Language specific prosodic preferences during the first half year of life: Evidence from German and French infants. Infant Behavior & Development, 32(3), 262–274. Crossref logo
Hoicka, E., & Wang, S. H.
(2011) Fifteen-month-old infants match vocal cues to intentional actions. Journal of Cognition and Development, 12(3), 299–314. Crossref logo
Hübscher, I., Esteve-Gibert, N., Igualada, A., & Prieto, P.
(2017) Intonation and gesture as bootstrapping devices in speaker uncertainty. First Language, 37(1), 24–41.Crossref logo
Igualada, A., Bosch, L., & Prieto, P.
(2015) Language development at 18 months is related to communicative strategies at 12 months. Infant Behavior and Development, 39, 42–52. Crossref logo
Iverson, J. M., & Goldin-Meadow, S.
(2005) Gesture paves the way for language development. Psychological Science, 16(5), 367–371. Crossref logo
Johnson, M. H., Dziurawiec, S., Ellis, H., & Morton, J.
(1991) Newborns preferential tracking of face-like stimuli and its subsequent decline. Cognition, 40(1–2), 1–19. Crossref logo
Kovács, Á. M., Téglás, E., & Endress, A. D.
(2010) The social sense: Susceptibility to others beliefs in human infants and adults. Science, 330, 1830–1834. Crossref logo
Ladd, D. R.
(1996) Intonational phonology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Lindová, J., Špinka, M., & Nováková, L.
(2015) Decoding of baby calls: Can adult humans identify the eliciting situation from emotional vocalizations of preverbal infants? PLoS ONE, 10(4), 1–16. Crossref logo
Liszkowski, U.
(2013) Using Theory of Mind. Child Development Perspectives, 7(2), 104–109. Crossref logo
(2014) Two sources of meaning in infant communication: Preceding action contexts and act-accompanying characteristics. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 369, 20130294. Crossref logo
Mampe, B., Friederici, A. D., Christophe, A., & Wermke, K.
(2009) Newborns cry melody is shaped by their native language. Current Biology, 19(23), 1994–1997. Crossref logo
Martin, A., Onishi, K. H., & Vouloumanos, A.
(2012) Understanding the abstract role of speech in communication at 12 months. Cognition, 123(1), 50–60. Crossref logo
Matthews, D.
(Ed.). (2014) Pragmatic development in first language acquisition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Crossref logo
McGillion, M., Herbert, J. S., Pine, J., Vihman, M. M., Depaolis, R., Keren-Portnoy, T., & Matthews, D.
(2017) What paves the way to conventional language? The predictive value of babble, pointing, and socioeconomic status. Child Development, 88(1), 156–166.Crossref logo
Meltzoff, A. N.
(1995) Understanding the intentions of others: Re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Developmental Psychology, 31(5), 838–850. Crossref logo
Miller, S. A.
(2009) Childrens’ understanding of second-order mental states. Psychological Bulletin, 135(5), 749–773. Crossref logo
Mumme, D. L., Fernald, A., & Herrera, C.
(1996) Infants’ responses to facial and vocal emotional signals in a social referencing paradigm. Child Development, 67, 3219–3237. Crossref logo
Murillo, E., & Capilla, A.
(2016) Properties of vocalization- and gesture-combinations in the transition to first words. Journal of Child Language, 43(4), 890–913.Crossref logo
Nazzi, T., Bertoncini, J., & Mehler, J.
(1998) Language discrimination by newborns: Toward an understanding of the role of rhythm. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 24(3), 756–766. Crossref logo
Oller, D. K., Buder, E. H., Ramsdell, H. L., Warlaumont, A. S., & Chorna, L.
(2013) Functional flexibility of infant vocalization and the emergence of language. PNAS, 110(16), 6318–6323. Crossref logo
Onishi, K. H., & Baillargeon, R.
(2005) Do 15-month-old infants understand false belief? Science, 308, 255–258. Crossref logo
Papaeliou, C. F., & Trevarthen, C.
(2006) Prelinguistic pitch patterns expressing “communication” and “apprehension”. Journal of Child Language, 33(1), 163–178. Crossref logo
Papaeliou, C., Minadakis, G., & Cavouras, D.
(2002) Acoustic patterns of infant vocalizations expressing emotions and communicative functions. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45(2), 311–317. Crossref logo
Papousek, M.
(1992) Early ontogeny of vocal communication in parent-infant interactions. In H. Papousek, U. Jurgens, & M. Papousek (Eds.), Nonverbal vocal communication: Comparative and developmental approaches (pp.230–261). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Portes, C., Beyssade, C., Michelas, A., Marandin, J. M., & Champagne-Lavau, M.
(2014) The dialogical dimension of intonational meaning: Evidence from French. Journal of Pragmatics, 74, 15–29. Crossref logo
Premack, D., & Woodruff, G.
(1978) Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 515–526. Crossref logo
Prieto, P.
(2015) Intonational meaning. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 6, 371–381.
Prieto, P., Estrella, A., Thorson, J., & Vanrell, M. M.
(2012) Is prosodic development correlated with grammatical and lexical development? Evidence from emerging intonation in Catalan and Spanish. Journal of Child Language, 39(2), 221–257. Crossref logo
Saint-Georges, C., Chetouani, M., Cassel, R., Apicella, F., Mahdhaoui, A., Muratori, F., Laznik, M. C., & Cohen, D.
(2013) Motherese in interaction: At the cross-road of emotion and cognition? (A systematic review). PloS One, 8(10), e78103. Crossref logo
Sakkalou, E., Ellis-Davies, K., Fowler, N. C., Hilbrink, E. E., & Gattis, M.
(2013) Infants show stability of goal-directed imitation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114(1), 1–9. Crossref logo
Sakkalou, E., & Gattis, M.
(2012) Infants infer intentions from prosody. Cognitive Development, 27(1), 1–16. Crossref logo
Scheiner, E., Hammerschmidt, K., Jürgens, U., & Zwirner, P.
(2002) Acoustic analyses of developmental changes and emotional expression in the preverbal vocalizations of infants. Journal of Voice, 16(4), 509–529. Crossref logo
Snow, D.
(2006) Regression and reorganization of intonation between 6 and 23 months. Child Development, 77(2), 281–296. Crossref logo
Sullivan, K., Zaitchik, D., Tager-Flusberg, H.
(1994) Preschoolers can attribute second-order beliefs. Developmental Psychology, 30, 395–402. Crossref logo
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., & Liszkowski, U.
(2007) A new look at infant pointing. Child Development, 78(3), 705–722. Crossref logo
Vaillant-Molina, M., Bahrick, L. E., & Flom, R.
(2013) Young infants match facial and vocal emotional expressions of other infants. Infancy, 18(1), 1–15.
Vouloumanos, A., Onishi, K. H., & Pogue, A.
(2012) Twelve-month-old infants recognize that speech can communicate unobservable intentions. PNAS, 109(32), 12933–12937. Crossref logo
Wermke, K., Teiser, J., Yovsi, D., Kohlenberg, P. J., Wermke, P., Robb, M., Keller, H., & Lamm, B.
(2016) Fundamental frequency variation within neonatal crying: Does ambient language matter? Speech , Language and Hearing, 19(4), 211–217.Crossref logo
Wilutzky, W.
(2015) Emotions as pragmatic and epistemic actions. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1593. Crossref logo
Woodward, A. L.
(1998) Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor’s reach. Cognition, 69(1), 1–34. Crossref logo
(1999) Infants ability to distinguish between purposeful and non-purposeful behaviors. Infant Behavior & Development, 22(2), 145–160. Crossref logo
Yeung, H. H., Chen, K. H., & Werker, J. F.
(2013) When does native language input affect phonetic perception? The precocious case of lexical tone. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(2), 123–139. Crossref logo
Zhang, D., Liu, Y., Hou, X., Sun, G., Cheng, Y., & Luo, Y.
(2014) Discrimination of fearful and angry emotional voices in sleeping human neonates: A study of the mismatch brain responses. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 422. Crossref logo