Early development of the prosody-meaning interface
Núria Esteve-Gibert | Aix Marseille Université, CNRS, LPL UMR
7309 | Universitat de Barcelona
Pilar Prieto | Institució Catalana de Recerca i Estudis Avançats
(ICREA) | Universitat Pompeu Fabra, Department of Translation
and Language Sciences
This chapter reviews evidence on how infants up to 18 months of age
develop the ability to use prosody as a sign of the expression of
pragmatic meanings, from both a comprehension and a production point
of view. Developmental research reveals that pre-lexical infants use
prosodic information not only to comprehend emotions in the speech
of their communicative partners, the intentional value of the
partners’ speech, and their speech act motivation, but also to
express these same pragmatic meanings when they communicate with
others. In essence, before the emergence of lexical and grammatical
skills, infants use prosody to communicate intentionally with the
world around them.
Article outline
Introduction
The prosody-meaning interface in infancy: Comprehension
Infants’ early understanding of others’ emotional states
through prosody
Early understanding of prosody as a marker of intentional
communication
Early understanding of prosody as a marker of speech act
information
The prosody-meaning interface in infancy: Production
Infants’ early use of prosody to mark their emotional status
in speech
Early production of prosody as a marker of intentional
communication
Early production of prosody as a marker of speech act
information
Armstrong, M. E. (2012). The development of yes-no question intonation in Puerto
Rican Spanish. Unpublished PhD dissertation. Ohio State University.
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “Theory of
Mind”?Cognition, 21, 37–46.
Bates, E., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1975). The acquisition of performatives prior to
speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly of Behavior and
Development, 21(3), 205–226.
Beaumont, S. L., & Bloom, K. (1993). Adults’ attributions of intentionality to
vocalizing infants. First Language, 13, 235–247.
Berman, J. M. J., Chambers, C. G., & Graham, S. A. (2016). Preschoolers’ real-time coordination of vocal and
facial emotional information. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 142, 391–399.
Blasi, A., Mercure, E., Lloyd-Fox, S., Thomson, A., Brammer, M., Sauter, D., Deeley, Q., Barker, G. J., Renvall, V., Deoni, S., Gasston, D., Williams, S. C. R., Johnson, M. H., Simmons, A., & Murphy, D. G. M. (2011). Early specialization for voice and emotion
processing in the infant brain. Current Biology, 21(14), 1220–1224.
Bruner, J. S. (1975). The ontogeny of speech acts. Journal of Child Language, 2(1), 1–19.
Cameron-Faulkner, T. (2014). The development of speech acts. In D. Matthews (Ed.), Pragmatic development in first language
acquisition (pp.37–52). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Carpenter, M., Akhtar, N., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Fourteen- through 18-month-old infants
differentially imitate intentional and accidental
actions. Infant Behavior & Development, 21(2), 315–330.
Clark, E. V. (2004). Pragmatics and language
acquisition. In L. Horn, & G. Ward (Eds.), The handbook of pragmatics (pp.562–577). Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2009). Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(4), 148–153.
Davis, B. L., MacNeilage, P. F., Matyear, C. L., & Powell, J. K. (2000). Prosodic correlates of stress in babbling: An
acoustical study. Child Development, 71(5), 1258–1270.
DeCasper, A. J., & Fifer, W. P. (1980). Of human bonding: Newborns prefer their mothers
voices. Science, 208,1174–1176.
De Villiers, J. (2007). The interface of language and Theory of
Mind. Lingua. International Review of General Linguistics.
Revue Internationale de Linguistique Générale, 117(11), 1858–1878.
Esteve-Gibert, N., Liszkowski, U., & Prieto, P. (2016). Prosodic and gestural features distinguish the
intention of pointing gestures in child-directed
communication. In M. E. Armstrong, N. Henriksen, & M. M. Vanrell (Eds.), Intonational grammar in Ibero-Romance. Approaches across
linguistic subfields (pp.249–276). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Esteve-Gibert, N., & Prieto P. (2013). Prosody signals the emergence of intentional
communication in the first year of life: Evidence from
Catalan-babbling infants. Journal of Child Language, 40(5), 919–944.
Esteve-Gibert, N., Prieto, P., & Liszkowski, U. (2017). Twelve-month-olds understand social intentions
based on prosody and gesture shape. Infancy, 22(1), 108–129. Doi:.
Fernald, A. (1993). Approval and disapproval: Infant responsiveness
to vocal affect in familiar and unfamiliar
languages. Child Development, 64, 657–674.
Fernald, A. (1985). Four-month-old infants prefer to listen to
motherese. Infant Behavior and Development, 8(2), 181–195.
Frota, S., Butler, J., & Vigário, M. (2014). Infants perception of intonation: Is it a
statement or a question?Infancy, 19(2), 194–213.
Goldin-Meadow, S., & Alibali, M. W. (2013). Gestures role in speaking, learning, and creating
language. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 257–283.
Goldstein, M. H., Schwade, J. A., & Bornstein, M. H. (2009). The value of vocalizing: Five-month-old infants
associate their own noncry vocalizations with responses from
caregivers. Child Development, 80(3), 636–644.
Grossmann, T., Oberecker, R., Koch, S. P., & Friederici, A. D. (2010). The developmental origins of voice processing in
the human brain. Neuron, 65(6), 852–858.
Grünloh, T., & Liszkowski, U. (2015. Prelinguistic vocalizations distinguish pointing
acts. Journal of Child Language, 42(6), 1312–1336.
Gussenhoven, C. (1984). On the grammar and semantics of sentence
accents. Dordrecht: Foris.
Höhle, B., Bijeljac-Babic, R., Herold, B., Weissenborn, J., & Nazzi, T. (2009). Language specific prosodic preferences during the
first half year of life: Evidence from German and French
infants. Infant Behavior & Development, 32(3), 262–274.
Hoicka, E., & Wang, S. H. (2011). Fifteen-month-old infants match vocal cues to
intentional actions. Journal of Cognition and Development, 12(3), 299–314.
Hübscher, I., Esteve-Gibert, N., Igualada, A., & Prieto, P. (2017). Intonation and gesture as bootstrapping devices
in speaker uncertainty. First Language, 37(1), 24–41.
Igualada, A., Bosch, L., & Prieto, P. (2015). Language development at 18 months is related to
communicative strategies at 12 months. Infant Behavior and Development, 39, 42–52.
Iverson, J. M., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). Gesture paves the way for language
development. Psychological Science, 16(5), 367–371.
Johnson, M. H., Dziurawiec, S., Ellis, H., & Morton, J. (1991). Newborns preferential tracking of face-like
stimuli and its subsequent decline. Cognition, 40(1–2), 1–19.
Kovács, Á. M., Téglás, E., & Endress, A. D. (2010). The social sense: Susceptibility to others
beliefs in human infants and adults. Science, 330, 1830–1834.
Ladd, D. R. (1996). Intonational phonology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Lindová, J., Špinka, M., & Nováková, L. (2015). Decoding of baby calls: Can adult humans identify
the eliciting situation from emotional vocalizations of
preverbal infants?PLoS ONE, 10(4), 1–16.
Liszkowski, U. (2013). Using Theory of Mind. Child Development Perspectives, 7(2), 104–109.
Liszkowski, U. (2014). Two sources of meaning in infant communication:
Preceding action contexts and act-accompanying
characteristics. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of
London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 369, 20130294.
Mampe, B., Friederici, A. D., Christophe, A., & Wermke, K. (2009). Newborns cry melody is shaped by their native
language. Current Biology, 19(23), 1994–1997.
Martin, A., Onishi, K. H., & Vouloumanos, A. (2012). Understanding the abstract role of speech in
communication at 12 months. Cognition, 123(1), 50–60.
McGillion, M., Herbert, J. S., Pine, J., Vihman, M. M., Depaolis, R., Keren-Portnoy, T., & Matthews, D. (2017). What paves the way to conventional language? The
predictive value of babble, pointing, and socioeconomic
status. Child Development, 88(1), 156–166.
Meltzoff, A. N. (1995). Understanding the intentions of others:
Re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old
children. Developmental Psychology, 31(5), 838–850.
Miller, S. A. (2009). Childrens’ understanding of second-order mental
states. Psychological Bulletin, 135(5), 749–773.
Mumme, D. L., Fernald, A., & Herrera, C. (1996). Infants’ responses to facial and vocal emotional
signals in a social referencing paradigm. Child Development, 67, 3219–3237.
Murillo, E., & Capilla, A. (2016). Properties of vocalization- and
gesture-combinations in the transition to first
words. Journal of Child Language, 43(4), 890–913.
Nazzi, T., Bertoncini, J., & Mehler, J. (1998). Language discrimination by newborns: Toward an
understanding of the role of rhythm. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and
Performance, 24(3), 756–766.
Oller, D. K., Buder, E. H., Ramsdell, H. L., Warlaumont, A. S., & Chorna, L. (2013). Functional flexibility of infant vocalization and
the emergence of language. PNAS, 110(16), 6318–6323.
Onishi, K. H., & Baillargeon, R. (2005). Do 15-month-old infants understand false
belief?Science, 308, 255–258.
Papaeliou, C. F., & Trevarthen, C. (2006). Prelinguistic pitch patterns expressing
“communication” and “apprehension”. Journal of Child Language, 33(1), 163–178.
Papaeliou, C., Minadakis, G., & Cavouras, D. (2002). Acoustic patterns of infant vocalizations
expressing emotions and communicative
functions. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing
Research, 45(2), 311–317.
Papousek, M. (1992). Early ontogeny of vocal communication in
parent-infant interactions. In H. Papousek, U. Jurgens, & M. Papousek (Eds.), Nonverbal vocal communication: Comparative and
developmental approaches (pp.230–261). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Portes, C., Beyssade, C., Michelas, A., Marandin, J. M., & Champagne-Lavau, M. (2014). The dialogical dimension of intonational meaning:
Evidence from French. Journal of Pragmatics, 74, 15–29.
Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of
mind?Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 515–526.
Prieto, P., Estrella, A., Thorson, J., & Vanrell, M. M. (2012). Is prosodic development correlated with
grammatical and lexical development? Evidence from emerging
intonation in Catalan and Spanish. Journal of Child Language, 39(2), 221–257.
Saint-Georges, C., Chetouani, M., Cassel, R., Apicella, F., Mahdhaoui, A., Muratori, F., Laznik, M. C., & Cohen, D. (2013). Motherese in interaction: At the cross-road of
emotion and cognition? (A systematic review). PloS One, 8(10), e78103.
Sakkalou, E., Ellis-Davies, K., Fowler, N. C., Hilbrink, E. E., & Gattis, M. (2013). Infants show stability of goal-directed
imitation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114(1), 1–9.
Sakkalou, E., & Gattis, M. (2012). Infants infer intentions from
prosody. Cognitive Development, 27(1), 1–16.
Scheiner, E., Hammerschmidt, K., Jürgens, U., & Zwirner, P. (2002). Acoustic analyses of developmental changes and
emotional expression in the preverbal vocalizations of
infants. Journal of Voice, 16(4), 509–529.
Snow, D. (2006). Regression and reorganization of intonation
between 6 and 23 months. Child Development, 77(2), 281–296.
Sullivan, K., Zaitchik, D., Tager-Flusberg, H. (1994). Preschoolers can attribute second-order
beliefs. Developmental Psychology, 30, 395–402.
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., & Liszkowski, U. (2007). A new look at infant pointing. Child Development, 78(3), 705–722.
Vaillant-Molina, M., Bahrick, L. E., & Flom, R. (2013). Young infants match facial and vocal emotional
expressions of other infants. Infancy, 18(1), 1–15.
Vouloumanos, A., Onishi, K. H., & Pogue, A. (2012). Twelve-month-old infants recognize that speech
can communicate unobservable intentions. PNAS, 109(32), 12933–12937.
Wermke, K., Teiser, J., Yovsi, D., Kohlenberg, P. J., Wermke, P., Robb, M., Keller, H., & Lamm, B. (2016). Fundamental frequency variation within neonatal
crying: Does ambient language matter?
Speech, Language and Hearing, 19(4), 211–217.
Wilutzky, W. (2015). Emotions as pragmatic and epistemic
actions. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1593.
Woodward, A. L. (1998). Infants selectively encode the goal object of an
actor’s reach. Cognition, 69(1), 1–34.
Woodward, A. L. (1999). Infants ability to distinguish between purposeful
and non-purposeful behaviors. Infant Behavior & Development, 22(2), 145–160.
Yeung, H. H., Chen, K. H., & Werker, J. F. (2013). When does native language input affect phonetic
perception? The precocious case of lexical
tone. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(2), 123–139.
Zhang, D., Liu, Y., Hou, X., Sun, G., Cheng, Y., & Luo, Y. (2014). Discrimination of fearful and angry emotional
voices in sleeping human neonates: A study of the mismatch
brain responses. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 422.
Cited by (5)
Cited by five other publications
Larrouy-Maestri, Pauline, David Poeppel & Marc D. Pell
2024. The Sound of Emotional Prosody: Nearly 3 Decades of Research and Future Directions. Perspectives on Psychological Science
2023. Expressive Pragmatics and Prosody in Young Preschoolers are More Closely Related to Structural Language than to Mentalizing. Language Learning and Development 19:3 ► pp. 323 ff.
Pronina, Mariia, Iris Hübscher, Ingrid Vilà-Giménez & Pilar Prieto
2021. Bridging the Gap Between Prosody and Pragmatics: The Acquisition of Pragmatic Prosody in the Preschool Years and Its Relation With Theory of Mind. Frontiers in Psychology 12
Rojas-Nieto, Cecilia
2021. Interactional sequences: child–adult collaboration in solving referring problems (Secuencias interlocutivas: colaboración adulto-niño en la solución de problemas referenciales). Journal for the Study of Education and Development 44:2 ► pp. 439 ff.
Hübscher, Iris, Laura Wagner & Pilar Prieto
2020. Three-year-olds infer polite stance from intonation and facial cues. Journal of Politeness Research 16:1 ► pp. 85 ff.
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 28 september 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.