Chapter published in:
The Development of Prosody in First Language Acquisition
Edited by Pilar Prieto and Núria Esteve-Gibert
[Trends in Language Acquisition Research 23] 2018
► pp. 227246
References

References

Armstrong, M. E.
(2012) The development of yes-no question intonation in Puerto Rican Spanish. Unpublished PhD dissertation. Ohio State University.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U.
(1985) Does the autistic child have a “Theory of Mind”? Cognition, 21, 37–46. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bates, E., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V.
(1975) The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly of Behavior and Development, 21(3), 205–226.Google Scholar
Beaumont, S. L., & Bloom, K.
(1993) Adults’ attributions of intentionality to vocalizing infants. First Language, 13, 235–247. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Berman, J. M. J., Chambers, C. G., & Graham, S. A.
(2016) Preschoolers’ real-time coordination of vocal and facial emotional information. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 142, 391–399. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Blasi, A., Mercure, E., Lloyd-Fox, S., Thomson, A., Brammer, M., Sauter, D., Deeley, Q., Barker, G. J., Renvall, V., Deoni, S., Gasston, D., Williams, S. C. R., Johnson, M. H., Simmons, A., & Murphy, D. G. M.
(2011) Early specialization for voice and emotion processing in the infant brain. Current Biology, 21(14), 1220–1224. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J. S.
(1975) The ontogeny of speech acts. Journal of Child Language, 2(1), 1–19. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cameron-Faulkner, T.
(2014) The development of speech acts. In D. Matthews (Ed.), Pragmatic development in first language acquisition (pp.37–52). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Carpenter, M., Akhtar, N., & Tomasello, M.
(1998) Fourteen- through 18-month-old infants differentially imitate intentional and accidental actions. Infant Behavior & Development, 21(2), 315–330. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Clark, E. V.
(2004) Pragmatics and language acquisition. In L. Horn, & G. Ward (Eds.), The handbook of pragmatics (pp.562–577). Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Csibra, G., & Gergely, G.
(2009) Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(4), 148–153. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Davis, B. L., MacNeilage, P. F., Matyear, C. L., & Powell, J. K.
(2000) Prosodic correlates of stress in babbling: An acoustical study. Child Development, 71(5), 1258–1270. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
DeCasper, A. J., & Fifer, W. P.
(1980) Of human bonding: Newborns prefer their mothers voices. Science, 208,1174–1176. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
De Villiers, J.
(2007) The interface of language and Theory of Mind. Lingua. International Review of General Linguistics. Revue Internationale de Linguistique Générale, 117(11), 1858–1878.Google Scholar
Esteve-Gibert, N., Liszkowski, U., & Prieto, P.
(2016) Prosodic and gestural features distinguish the intention of pointing gestures in child-directed communication. In M. E. Armstrong, N. Henriksen, & M. M. Vanrell (Eds.), Intonational grammar in Ibero-Romance. Approaches across linguistic subfields (pp.249–276). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Esteve-Gibert, N., & Prieto P.
(2013) Prosody signals the emergence of intentional communication in the first year of life: Evidence from Catalan-babbling infants. Journal of Child Language, 40(5), 919–944. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Esteve-Gibert, N., Prieto, P., & Liszkowski, U.
(2017) Twelve-month-olds understand social intentions based on prosody and gesture shape. Infancy, 22(1), 108–129. Doi:Crossref.Google Scholar
Fernald, A.
(1993) Approval and disapproval: Infant responsiveness to vocal affect in familiar and unfamiliar languages. Child Development, 64, 657–674. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1985) Four-month-old infants prefer to listen to motherese. Infant Behavior and Development, 8(2), 181–195. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Frota, S., Butler, J., & Vigário, M.
(2014) Infants perception of intonation: Is it a statement or a question? Infancy, 19(2), 194–213. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S., & Alibali, M. W.
(2013) Gestures role in speaking, learning, and creating language. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 257–283. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Goldstein, M. H., Schwade, J. A., & Bornstein, M. H.
(2009) The value of vocalizing: Five-month-old infants associate their own noncry vocalizations with responses from caregivers. Child Development, 80(3), 636–644. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Grossmann, T., Oberecker, R., Koch, S. P., & Friederici, A. D.
(2010) The developmental origins of voice processing in the human brain. Neuron, 65(6), 852–858. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Grünloh, T., & Liszkowski, U.
(2015Prelinguistic vocalizations distinguish pointing acts. Journal of Child Language, 42(6), 1312–1336. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gussenhoven, C.
(1984) On the grammar and semantics of sentence accents. Dordrecht: Foris.CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Höhle, B., Bijeljac-Babic, R., Herold, B., Weissenborn, J., & Nazzi, T.
(2009) Language specific prosodic preferences during the first half year of life: Evidence from German and French infants. Infant Behavior & Development, 32(3), 262–274. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hoicka, E., & Wang, S. H.
(2011) Fifteen-month-old infants match vocal cues to intentional actions. Journal of Cognition and Development, 12(3), 299–314. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hübscher, I., Esteve-Gibert, N., Igualada, A., & Prieto, P.
(2017) Intonation and gesture as bootstrapping devices in speaker uncertainty. First Language, 37(1), 24–41.CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Igualada, A., Bosch, L., & Prieto, P.
(2015) Language development at 18 months is related to communicative strategies at 12 months. Infant Behavior and Development, 39, 42–52. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Iverson, J. M., & Goldin-Meadow, S.
(2005) Gesture paves the way for language development. Psychological Science, 16(5), 367–371. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, M. H., Dziurawiec, S., Ellis, H., & Morton, J.
(1991) Newborns preferential tracking of face-like stimuli and its subsequent decline. Cognition, 40(1–2), 1–19. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kovács, Á. M., Téglás, E., & Endress, A. D.
(2010) The social sense: Susceptibility to others beliefs in human infants and adults. Science, 330, 1830–1834. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ladd, D. R.
(1996) Intonational phonology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lindová, J., Špinka, M., & Nováková, L.
(2015) Decoding of baby calls: Can adult humans identify the eliciting situation from emotional vocalizations of preverbal infants? PLoS ONE, 10(4), 1–16. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Liszkowski, U.
(2013) Using Theory of Mind. Child Development Perspectives, 7(2), 104–109. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2014) Two sources of meaning in infant communication: Preceding action contexts and act-accompanying characteristics. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 369, 20130294. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mampe, B., Friederici, A. D., Christophe, A., & Wermke, K.
(2009) Newborns cry melody is shaped by their native language. Current Biology, 19(23), 1994–1997. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Martin, A., Onishi, K. H., & Vouloumanos, A.
(2012) Understanding the abstract role of speech in communication at 12 months. Cognition, 123(1), 50–60. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Matthews, D.
(Ed.). (2014) Pragmatic development in first language acquisition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossrefGoogle Scholar
McGillion, M., Herbert, J. S., Pine, J., Vihman, M. M., Depaolis, R., Keren-Portnoy, T., & Matthews, D.
(2017) What paves the way to conventional language? The predictive value of babble, pointing, and socioeconomic status. Child Development, 88(1), 156–166.CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N.
(1995) Understanding the intentions of others: Re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Developmental Psychology, 31(5), 838–850. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Miller, S. A.
(2009) Childrens’ understanding of second-order mental states. Psychological Bulletin, 135(5), 749–773. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mumme, D. L., Fernald, A., & Herrera, C.
(1996) Infants’ responses to facial and vocal emotional signals in a social referencing paradigm. Child Development, 67, 3219–3237. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Murillo, E., & Capilla, A.
(2016) Properties of vocalization- and gesture-combinations in the transition to first words. Journal of Child Language, 43(4), 890–913.CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nazzi, T., Bertoncini, J., & Mehler, J.
(1998) Language discrimination by newborns: Toward an understanding of the role of rhythm. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 24(3), 756–766. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Oller, D. K., Buder, E. H., Ramsdell, H. L., Warlaumont, A. S., & Chorna, L.
(2013) Functional flexibility of infant vocalization and the emergence of language. PNAS, 110(16), 6318–6323. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Onishi, K. H., & Baillargeon, R.
(2005) Do 15-month-old infants understand false belief? Science, 308, 255–258. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Papaeliou, C. F., & Trevarthen, C.
(2006) Prelinguistic pitch patterns expressing “communication” and “apprehension”. Journal of Child Language, 33(1), 163–178. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Papaeliou, C., Minadakis, G., & Cavouras, D.
(2002) Acoustic patterns of infant vocalizations expressing emotions and communicative functions. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45(2), 311–317. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Papousek, M.
(1992) Early ontogeny of vocal communication in parent-infant interactions. In H. Papousek, U. Jurgens, & M. Papousek (Eds.), Nonverbal vocal communication: Comparative and developmental approaches (pp.230–261). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Portes, C., Beyssade, C., Michelas, A., Marandin, J. M., & Champagne-Lavau, M.
(2014) The dialogical dimension of intonational meaning: Evidence from French. Journal of Pragmatics, 74, 15–29. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Premack, D., & Woodruff, G.
(1978) Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 515–526. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Prieto, P.
(2015) Intonational meaning. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 6, 371–381.Google Scholar
Prieto, P., Estrella, A., Thorson, J., & Vanrell, M. M.
(2012) Is prosodic development correlated with grammatical and lexical development? Evidence from emerging intonation in Catalan and Spanish. Journal of Child Language, 39(2), 221–257. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Saint-Georges, C., Chetouani, M., Cassel, R., Apicella, F., Mahdhaoui, A., Muratori, F., Laznik, M. C., & Cohen, D.
(2013) Motherese in interaction: At the cross-road of emotion and cognition? (A systematic review). PloS One, 8(10), e78103. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sakkalou, E., Ellis-Davies, K., Fowler, N. C., Hilbrink, E. E., & Gattis, M.
(2013) Infants show stability of goal-directed imitation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114(1), 1–9. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sakkalou, E., & Gattis, M.
(2012) Infants infer intentions from prosody. Cognitive Development, 27(1), 1–16. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Scheiner, E., Hammerschmidt, K., Jürgens, U., & Zwirner, P.
(2002) Acoustic analyses of developmental changes and emotional expression in the preverbal vocalizations of infants. Journal of Voice, 16(4), 509–529. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Snow, D.
(2006) Regression and reorganization of intonation between 6 and 23 months. Child Development, 77(2), 281–296. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sullivan, K., Zaitchik, D., Tager-Flusberg, H.
(1994) Preschoolers can attribute second-order beliefs. Developmental Psychology, 30, 395–402. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., & Liszkowski, U.
(2007) A new look at infant pointing. Child Development, 78(3), 705–722. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Vaillant-Molina, M., Bahrick, L. E., & Flom, R.
(2013) Young infants match facial and vocal emotional expressions of other infants. Infancy, 18(1), 1–15.Google Scholar
Vouloumanos, A., Onishi, K. H., & Pogue, A.
(2012) Twelve-month-old infants recognize that speech can communicate unobservable intentions. PNAS, 109(32), 12933–12937. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wermke, K., Teiser, J., Yovsi, D., Kohlenberg, P. J., Wermke, P., Robb, M., Keller, H., & Lamm, B.
(2016) Fundamental frequency variation within neonatal crying: Does ambient language matter? Speech , Language and Hearing, 19(4), 211–217.CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wilutzky, W.
(2015) Emotions as pragmatic and epistemic actions. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1593. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Woodward, A. L.
(1998) Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor’s reach. Cognition, 69(1), 1–34. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1999) Infants ability to distinguish between purposeful and non-purposeful behaviors. Infant Behavior & Development, 22(2), 145–160. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Yeung, H. H., Chen, K. H., & Werker, J. F.
(2013) When does native language input affect phonetic perception? The precocious case of lexical tone. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(2), 123–139. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Zhang, D., Liu, Y., Hou, X., Sun, G., Cheng, Y., & Luo, Y.
(2014) Discrimination of fearful and angry emotional voices in sleeping human neonates: A study of the mismatch brain responses. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 422. CrossrefGoogle Scholar