Children exhibit sophisticated mental state (epistemic) reasoning abilities from an early age, but it remains unclear what role these abilities play in the development of pragmatic inference in language acquisition. Here, we examined the role of epistemic reasoning in scalar implicature. Experiment 1 found that most 4-year-olds successfully computed a sub-type of scalar implicature (ad hoc implicatures), despite failing to compute so-called “ignorance implicatures”, a type of epistemic inference that Gricean models of pragmatics deem necessary for scalar implicature. In Experiment 2, we tested 4- and 5-year-old children, and replicated the finding that children compute ad hoc implicatures more readily than ignorance implicatures, and that children’s difficulties with ignorance implicature are unrelated to performance on Theory of Mind tasks.
Baillargeon, R., Scott, R. M., & He, Z. (2010). False-belief understanding in infants. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 110–118.
Baldwin, D. A. (1991). Infants’ contribution to the achievement of joint reference. Child Development, 62, 874–890.
Bale, A., & Barner, D. (2013). Grammatical alternatives and pragmatic development. In A. Falaus (Ed.), Alternatives in semantics. Houndmills: Palgrave.
Barner, D., & Bachrach, A. (2010). Inference and exact numerical representation in early language development. Cognitive Psychology, 60, 40–62.
Barner, D., Brooks, N., & Bale, A. (2011). Accessing the unsaid: The role of scalar alternatives in children’s pragmatic inference. Cognition, 188, 87–96.
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2014). lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4. R package version, 1(7), 1–23.
Bates, D., Kliegl, R., Vasishth, S., & Baayen, H. (2015). Parsimonious mixed models. arXiv preprint arXiv:1506.04967.
Bates, D., Kliegl, R., Vasishth, S., & Baayen, R. H. (2015a). Parsimonious mixed models. arXiv:1506.04967.
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S (2015b). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1–48.
Behne, T., Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2005). Unwilling versus unable: infants’ understanding of intentional action. Developmental Psychology, 41, 328–337.
Bloom, P. (2000). How children learn the meanings of words. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Braine, M. D. S., & Rumain, B. (1983). Logical reasoning. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol. III: Cognitive development (4th ed., J. H. Flavell & E. M. Markman (Vol. Eds.), pp. 263–340). New York, NY: John Wiley.
Carpenter, M., Akhtar, N., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Fourteen- to 18-month-old infants differentially imitate intentional and accidental actions. Infant Behavior and Development, 21, 315–330.
Chierchia, G. (2004). Scalar implicatures, polarity phenomena, and the syntax/pragmatics interface. In A. Belletti (Ed.), Structures and beyond (pp. 39–103). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Chierchia, G. (2006). Broaden your views: Implicatures of domain widening and the “locality” of language. Linguistic Inquiry, 37, 535–590.
Chierchia, G., Crain, S., Guasti, M. T., Gualmini, A., & Meroni, L. (2001). The acquisition of disjunction: Evidence for a grammatical view of scalar implicatures. In A. H-J. Do, L. Domínguez, & A. Johansen (Eds.), BUCLD 25: Proceedings of the 25th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 157–168). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
Chierchia, G., Fox, D., & Spector, B. (2012). The grammatical view of scalar implicatures and the relationship between syntax and semantics. In C. Maienborn, K. von Heusinger, & P. Portner. (Eds.), Semantics: An international handbook of natural language meaning (Vol. 1). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Clark, E. (1990). On the pragmatics of contrast. Journal of Child Language, 17, 417–431.
de Marchena, A., Eigsti, I. M., Worek, A., Ono, K. E., & Snedeker, J. (2011). Mutual exclusivity in autism spectrum disorders: Testing the pragmatic hypothesis. Cognition, 119, 96–113.
Diesendruck, G., & Markson, L. (2001). Children’s avoidance of lexical overlap: A pragmatic account. Developmental Psychology, 37, 630–641.
Foppolo, F., Guasti, M. T., & Chierchia, G. (2012). Scalar implicatures in child language: Give children a chance. Language Learning and Development, 8, 365–394.
Fox, D. (2007). Free choice and the theory of scalar implicatures. In U. Sauerland & P. Stateva (Eds.), Presupposition and implicature in compositional semantics. Houndmills: Palgrave Macmillan.
Fox, D. & Hackl, M. (2006). The universal density of measurement. Linguistics and Philosophy, 29, 537–586.
Gathercole, V. C. (1989). Contrast: A semantic constraint?Journal of Child Language, 16, 685–702.
Gazdar, G. (1979). Pragmatics: Implicature, presupposition, and logical form. New York, NY: Academic Press.
Geurts, B. (2009). Scalar implicature and local pragmatics. Mind and Language, 24, 51–79.
Grice, P. (1975). Logic and conversation. In P. Cole & J. Morgan (Eds.), Syntax and Semantics (Vol. 3). New York, NY: Academic Press.
Gualmini, A., Crain, S., Meroni, L., Chierchia, G., & Guasti, M. T. (2001). At the semantics/pragmatics interface in child language. In R. Hastings, B. Jackson, & Z. Zvolenszky (Eds.), Proceedings of Semantics and Linguistic Theory XI. Ithaca, NY: CLC Publications.
Hirschberg, J. (1985). A theory of scalar implicature (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Pennsylvania.
Hochstein, L., Bale, A., Fox, D., & Barner, D. (2016). Ignorance and inference: Do problems with Gricean epistemic reasoning explain children’s difficulty with scalar implicature?Journal of Semantics, 33(1), 107–135.
Horn, L. (1972). On the semantic properties of logical operators in English (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of California, Los Angeles, CA.
Katsos, N., & Bishop, D. (2011). Pragmatic tolerance: Implications for the acquisition of informativeness and implicature. Cognition, 120, 67–81.
Katzir, R. (2007). Structurally-defined alternatives. Linguistics and Philosophy, 30, 669–690.
Landman, F. (1998). Plurals and maximalization. In S. Rothstein (Ed.), Events and grammar (pp. 237–271). Dordrecht: Kluwer.
Levinson, S. (2000). Presumptive meaning. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.
Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Twelve-month-olds communicate helpfully and appropriately for knowledgeable and ignorant partners. Cognition, 108, 732–739.
Markman, E. M., & Wachtel, G. F. (1988). Children’s use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meanings of words. Cognitive Psychology, 20, 121–157.
Markman, E. M., Wasow, J. L., & Hansen, M. B. (2003). Use of the mutual exclusivity assumption by young word learners. Cognitive psychology, 47(3), 241–275.
Meyer, M. (2013). Ignorance and grammar (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). MIT.
Miller, K., Schmitt, C., Chang, H., & Munn, A. (2005). Young children understand some implicatures. In A. Brugos, M. Clark-Cotton, & S. Ha (Eds.), BUCLD 29: Proceedings of the 29th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 389–400). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
Noveck, I. (2001). When children are more logical than adults: Experimental investigations of scalar implicature. Cognition, 78, 165–188.
O’Neill, D. K. (1996). Two-year-old children’s sensitivity to a parent’s knowledge state when making requests. Child Development, 67, 659–677.
Onishi, K. H., & Baillargeon, R. (2005). Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs?Science, 308, 255–258.
Papafragou, A., & Musolino, J. (2003). Scalar implicatures: Experiments at the semantics-pragmatics interface. Cognition, 86, 253–282.
Papafragou, A., & Tantalou, N. (2004). Children’s computation of implicatures. Language Acquisition, 12, 71–82.
Paris, S. (1973). Comprehension of language connectives and propositional logical relationships. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 16, 278–291.
Pouscoulous, N., Noveck, I., Politzer, G., & Bastide, A. (2007). A developmental investigation of processing costs in implicature production. Language Acquisition, 14, 347–375.
Preissler, M. A., & Carey, S. (2005). The role of inferences about referential intent in word learning: Evidence from autism. Cognition, 97, B13–B23.
R Core Team (2014). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. <
[URL]>
Reinhart, T. (2004). The processing cost of reference-set computation: Acquisition of stress shift and focus. Language Acquisition, 12, 109–155.
Rooth, M. (1992). A theory of focus interpretation. Natural Language Semantics, 1(1), 117–121.
Russell, B. (2011). Topics in the computation of scalar implicatures. PhD diss., Brown University. [URL]. net/Archive/WY1YTRhM/russell2012probabilistic. pdf.
Russell, B. (2011). Topics in the computation of scalar implicatures (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Brown University. Posted on Semantics Archive: <[URL]>
Sabbagh, M. A., & Baldwin, D. A. (2001). Learning words from knowledgeable versus ignorant speakers: Links between preschoolers’ theory of mind and semantic development. Child Development, 72, 1054–1070.
Sauerland, U. (2004). Scalar implicatures in complex sentences. Linguistics and Philosophy, 27, 367–391
Skordos, D., & Papafragou, A. (2016). Children’s derivation of scalar implicatures: Alternatives and relevance. Cognition, 153, 6–18.
Smith, C. (1980). Quantifiers and question answering in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 30, 191–205.
Southgate, V., Senju, A., & Csibra, G. (2007). Action anticipation through attribution of false belief by 2-year-olds. Psychological Science, 18, 587–592.
Stiller, A., Goodman, N. D., & Frank, M. C. (2011). Ad-hoc scalar implicature in adults and children. In L. Carlson (Ed.), Proceedings of the 33rd annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, Boston, July.
Stiller, A. J., Goodman, N. D., & Frank, M. C. (2015). Ad-hoc implicature in preschool children. Language Learning and Development, 11(2), 176–190.
Sullivan, J., Davidson, K., & Barner, D. (2011). Children’s conversational implicatures. Paper presented at the 36th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, Boston, MA.
Sullivan, J.; Davidson, K.; Wade, S.; and Barner, D. (under review). Differentiating scalar implicature from mutual exclusivity in language acquisition.
Surian, L., Caldi, S., & Sperber, D. (2007). Attribution of beliefs by 13-month-old infants. Psychological Science, 18, 580–586.
Syrett, K., & Arunachalam, S. (2016). Young children’s developing expectations about the language of events. In J. A. Scott & D. Waughtal (Eds.), BUCLD 40: Proceedings of the 40th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 375–390). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
Tomasello, M., & Barton, M. E. (1994). Learning words in nonostensive contexts. Developmental Psychology, 30, 639–650.
Wellman, H. M., & Liu, D. (2004). Scaling of Theory-of-Mind tasks. Child Development, 75, 523–541.
Woodward, A. L., & Markman, E. M. (1998). Early word learning. In W. Damon, D. Kuhn, & R. Siegler (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 2: Cognition, perception, and language (pp. 371–420). New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.
Wynn, K. (1992). Children’s acquisition of the number words and the counting system. Cognitive psychology, 24(2), 220–251.
Cited by (7)
Cited by seven other publications
Katsos, Napoleon, Blanche Gonzales de Linares, Ekaterina Ostashchenko & Elspeth Wilson
2023. Perspective-taking in deriving implicatures: The listener's perspective is important too. Cognition 241 ► pp. 105582 ff.
Wilson, Elspeth, Rebecca Lawrence & Napoleon Katsos
2023. The Role of Perspective-Taking in Children’s Quantity Implicatures. Language Learning and Development 19:2 ► pp. 167 ff.
Anna Papafragou, John C. Trueswell & Lila R. Gleitman
2022. The Oxford Handbook of the Mental Lexicon,
Sullivan, Jessica, Joseph Alvarez & Blair Goldstein
2022. Preschoolers’ relevance inferences in linguistic and non‐linguistic contexts. Developmental Science 25:2
WILSON, Elspeth & Napoleon KATSOS
2022. Pragmatic, linguistic and cognitive factors in young children's development of quantity, relevance and word learning inferences. Journal of Child Language 49:6 ► pp. 1065 ff.
Gotzner, Nicole, Barner, David & Crain, Stephen
2020. Disjunction Triggers Exhaustivity Implicatures in 4- to 5-Year-Olds: Investigating the Role of Access to Alternatives. Journal of Semantics 37:2 ► pp. 219 ff.
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 28 september 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.