Part of
Semantics in Language Acquisition
Edited by Kristen Syrett and Sudha Arunachalam
[Trends in Language Acquisition Research 24] 2018
► pp. 326349
References
Baillargeon, R., Scott, R. M., & He, Z.
(2010) False-belief understanding in infants. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 110–118.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baldwin, D. A.
(1991) Infants’ contribution to the achievement of joint reference. Child Development, 62, 874–890.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bale, A., & Barner, D.
(2013) Grammatical alternatives and pragmatic development. In A. Falaus (Ed.), Alternatives in semantics. Houndmills: Palgrave.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barner, D., & Bachrach, A.
(2010) Inference and exact numerical representation in early language development. Cognitive Psychology, 60, 40–62.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barner, D., Brooks, N., & Bale, A.
(2011) Accessing the unsaid: The role of scalar alternatives in children’s pragmatic inference. Cognition, 188, 87–96.Google Scholar
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S.
(2014) lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4. R package version, 1(7), 1–23.Google Scholar
Bates, D., Kliegl, R., Vasishth, S., & Baayen, H.
(2015) Parsimonious mixed models. arXiv preprint arXiv:1506.04967.Google Scholar
Bates, D., Kliegl, R., Vasishth, S., & Baayen, R. H.
(2015a) Parsimonious mixed models. arXiv:1506.04967.Google Scholar
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S
(2015b) Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1–48.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Behne, T., Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M.
(2005) Unwilling versus unable: infants’ understanding of intentional action. Developmental Psychology, 41, 328–337.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bloom, P.
(2000) How children learn the meanings of words. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Braine, M. D. S., & Rumain, B.
(1983) Logical reasoning. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol. III: Cognitive development (4th ed., J. H. Flavell & E. M. Markman (Vol. Eds.), pp. 263–340). New York, NY: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Carpenter, M., Akhtar, N., & Tomasello, M.
(1998) Fourteen- to 18-month-old infants differentially imitate intentional and accidental actions. Infant Behavior and Development, 21, 315–330.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chierchia, G.
(2004) Scalar implicatures, polarity phenomena, and the syntax/pragmatics interface. In A. Belletti (Ed.), Structures and beyond (pp. 39–103). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
(2006) Broaden your views: Implicatures of domain widening and the “locality” of language. Linguistic Inquiry, 37, 535–590.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chierchia, G., Crain, S., Guasti, M. T., Gualmini, A., & Meroni, L.
(2001) The acquisition of disjunction: Evidence for a grammatical view of scalar implicatures. In A. H-J. Do, L. Domínguez, & A. Johansen (Eds.), BUCLD 25: Proceedings of the 25th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 157–168). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Chierchia, G., Fox, D., & Spector, B.
(2012) The grammatical view of scalar implicatures and the relationship between syntax and semantics. In C. Maienborn, K. von Heusinger, & P. Portner. (Eds.), Semantics: An international handbook of natural language meaning (Vol. 1). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Clark, E.
(1990) On the pragmatics of contrast. Journal of Child Language, 17, 417–431.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
de Marchena, A., Eigsti, I. M., Worek, A., Ono, K. E., & Snedeker, J.
(2011) Mutual exclusivity in autism spectrum disorders: Testing the pragmatic hypothesis. Cognition, 119, 96–113.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Diesendruck, G., & Markson, L.
(2001) Children’s avoidance of lexical overlap: A pragmatic account. Developmental Psychology, 37, 630–641.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Foppolo, F., Guasti, M. T., & Chierchia, G.
(2012) Scalar implicatures in child language: Give children a chance. Language Learning and Development, 8, 365–394.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fox, D.
(2007) Free choice and the theory of scalar implicatures. In U. Sauerland & P. Stateva (Eds.), Presupposition and implicature in compositional semantics. Houndmills: Palgrave Macmillan.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fox, D. & Hackl, M.
(2006) The universal density of measurement. Linguistics and Philosophy, 29, 537–586.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, V. C.
(1989) Contrast: A semantic constraint? Journal of Child Language, 16, 685–702.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gazdar, G.
(1979) Pragmatics: Implicature, presupposition, and logical form. New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Geurts, B.
(2009) Scalar implicature and local pragmatics. Mind and Language, 24, 51–79.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grice, P.
(1975) Logic and conversation. In P. Cole & J. Morgan (Eds.), Syntax and Semantics (Vol. 3). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Gualmini, A., Crain, S., Meroni, L., Chierchia, G., & Guasti, M. T.
(2001) At the semantics/pragmatics interface in child language. In R. Hastings, B. Jackson, & Z. Zvolenszky (Eds.), Proceedings of Semantics and Linguistic Theory XI. Ithaca, NY: CLC Publications.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hirschberg, J.
(1985) A theory of scalar implicature (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
Hochstein, L., Bale, A., Fox, D., & Barner, D.
(2016) Ignorance and inference: Do problems with Gricean epistemic reasoning explain children’s difficulty with scalar implicature? Journal of Semantics, 33(1), 107–135.Google Scholar
Horn, L.
(1972) On the semantic properties of logical operators in English (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of California, Los Angeles, CA.Google Scholar
Katsos, N., & Bishop, D.
(2011) Pragmatic tolerance: Implications for the acquisition of informativeness and implicature. Cognition, 120, 67–81.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Katzir, R.
(2007) Structurally-defined alternatives. Linguistics and Philosophy, 30, 669–690.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Landman, F.
(1998) Plurals and maximalization. In S. Rothstein (Ed.), Events and grammar (pp. 237–271). Dordrecht: Kluwer.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Levinson, S.
(2000) Presumptive meaning. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M.
(2008) Twelve-month-olds communicate helpfully and appropriately for knowledgeable and ignorant partners. Cognition, 108, 732–739.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Markman, E. M., & Wachtel, G. F.
(1988) Children’s use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meanings of words. Cognitive Psychology, 20, 121–157.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Markman, E. M., Wasow, J. L., & Hansen, M. B.
(2003) Use of the mutual exclusivity assumption by young word learners. Cognitive psychology, 47(3), 241–275.Google Scholar
Meyer, M.
(2013) Ignorance and grammar (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). MIT.Google Scholar
Miller, K., Schmitt, C., Chang, H., & Munn, A.
(2005) Young children understand some implicatures. In A. Brugos, M. Clark-Cotton, & S. Ha (Eds.), BUCLD 29: Proceedings of the 29th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 389–400). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Noveck, I.
(2001) When children are more logical than adults: Experimental investigations of scalar implicature. Cognition, 78, 165–188.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
O’Neill, D. K.
(1996) Two-year-old children’s sensitivity to a parent’s knowledge state when making requests. Child Development, 67, 659–677.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Onishi, K. H., & Baillargeon, R.
(2005) Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science, 308, 255–258.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Papafragou, A., & Musolino, J.
(2003) Scalar implicatures: Experiments at the semantics-pragmatics interface. Cognition, 86, 253–282.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Papafragou, A., & Tantalou, N.
(2004) Children’s computation of implicatures. Language Acquisition, 12, 71–82.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paris, S.
(1973) Comprehension of language connectives and propositional logical relationships. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 16, 278–291.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pouscoulous, N., Noveck, I., Politzer, G., & Bastide, A.
(2007) A developmental investigation of processing costs in implicature production. Language Acquisition, 14, 347–375.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Preissler, M. A., & Carey, S.
(2005) The role of inferences about referential intent in word learning: Evidence from autism. Cognition, 97, B13–B23.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
R Core Team
(2014) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. < [URL]>
Reinhart, T.
(2004) The processing cost of reference-set computation: Acquisition of stress shift and focus. Language Acquisition, 12, 109–155.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rooth, M.
(1992) A theory of focus interpretation. Natural Language Semantics, 1(1), 117–121.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Russell, B.
(2011) Topics in the computation of scalar implicatures. PhD diss., Brown University. [URL]. net/Archive/WY1YTRhM/russell2012probabilistic. pdf.Google Scholar
(2011) Topics in the computation of scalar implicatures (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Brown University. Posted on Semantics Archive: <[URL]>
Sabbagh, M. A., & Baldwin, D. A.
(2001) Learning words from knowledgeable versus ignorant speakers: Links between preschoolers’ theory of mind and semantic development. Child Development, 72, 1054–1070.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sauerland, U.
(2004) Scalar implicatures in complex sentences. Linguistics and Philosophy, 27, 367–391 DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Skordos, D., & Papafragou, A.
(2016) Children’s derivation of scalar implicatures: Alternatives and relevance. Cognition, 153, 6–18.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smith, C.
(1980) Quantifiers and question answering in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 30, 191–205.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Southgate, V., Senju, A., & Csibra, G.
(2007) Action anticipation through attribution of false belief by 2-year-olds. Psychological Science, 18, 587–592.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stiller, A., Goodman, N. D., & Frank, M. C.
(2011) Ad-hoc scalar implicature in adults and children. In L. Carlson (Ed.), Proceedings of the 33rd annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, Boston, July.Google Scholar
Stiller, A. J., Goodman, N. D., & Frank, M. C.
(2015) Ad-hoc implicature in preschool children. Language Learning and Development, 11(2), 176–190.Google Scholar
Sullivan, J., Davidson, K., & Barner, D.
(2011) Children’s conversational implicatures. Paper presented at the 36th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, Boston, MA.
Sullivan, J.; Davidson, K.; Wade, S.; and Barner, D.
(under review) Differentiating scalar implicature from mutual exclusivity in language acquisition.Google Scholar
Surian, L., Caldi, S., & Sperber, D.
(2007) Attribution of beliefs by 13-month-old infants. Psychological Science, 18, 580–586.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Syrett, K., & Arunachalam, S.
(2016) Young children’s developing expectations about the language of events. In J. A. Scott & D. Waughtal (Eds.), BUCLD 40: Proceedings of the 40th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 375–390). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Barton, M. E.
(1994) Learning words in nonostensive contexts. Developmental Psychology, 30, 639–650.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wellman, H. M., & Liu, D.
(2004) Scaling of Theory-of-Mind tasks. Child Development, 75, 523–541.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Woodward, A. L., & Markman, E. M.
(1998) Early word learning. In W. Damon, D. Kuhn, & R. Siegler (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 2: Cognition, perception, and language (pp. 371–420). New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Wynn, K.
(1992) Children’s acquisition of the number words and the counting system. Cognitive psychology, 24(2), 220–251.Google Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 7 other publications

Gotzner, Nicole, Barner, David & Crain, Stephen
2020. Disjunction Triggers Exhaustivity Implicatures in 4- to 5-Year-Olds: Investigating the Role of Access to Alternatives. Journal of Semantics 37:2  pp. 219 ff. DOI logo
Kampa, Alyssa & Anna Papafragou
2020. Four‐year‐olds incorporate speaker knowledge into pragmatic inferences. Developmental Science 23:3 DOI logo
Katsos, Napoleon, Blanche Gonzales de Linares, Ekaterina Ostashchenko & Elspeth Wilson
2023. Perspective-taking in deriving implicatures: The listener's perspective is important too. Cognition 241  pp. 105582 ff. DOI logo
Anna Papafragou, John C. Trueswell & Lila R. Gleitman
2022. The Oxford Handbook of the Mental Lexicon, DOI logo
Sullivan, Jessica, Joseph Alvarez & Blair Goldstein
2022. Preschoolers’ relevance inferences in linguistic and non‐linguistic contexts. Developmental Science 25:2 DOI logo
WILSON, Elspeth & Napoleon KATSOS
2022. Pragmatic, linguistic and cognitive factors in young children's development of quantity, relevance and word learning inferences. Journal of Child Language 49:6  pp. 1065 ff. DOI logo
Wilson, Elspeth, Rebecca Lawrence & Napoleon Katsos
2023. The Role of Perspective-Taking in Children’s Quantity Implicatures. Language Learning and Development 19:2  pp. 167 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 22 april 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.