References

References

Agrafiotis, D., Canagarajah, N., Bull, D. R., & Dye, M.
(2003) Perceptually optimised sign language video coding based on eye tracking analysis. Electronics Letters 39(24), 1703–1705. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Agrafiotis, D., Canagarajah, N., Bull, D. R., Kyle, J., Seers, H., & Dye, M.
(2006) A perceptually optimised video coding system for sign language communication at low bit rates. Signal Processing: Image Communication 21(7), 531–549.Google Scholar
Baker-Shenk, C.
(1983) A micro-analysis of the non-manual components of questions in American Sign Language. Berkeley, CA: University of California.Google Scholar
Battison, R.
(1978) Lexical borrowing in American Sign Language. Silver Spring, MD: Linstok Press.Google Scholar
Bellugi, U., & Klima, E.
(1991) What the hands reveal about the brain. In D. S. Martin (Ed.), Advances in cognition, education and deafness. Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.Google Scholar
Bosworth, R. G., & Dobkins, K. R.
(2002) Visual field asymmetries for motion processing in deaf and hearing signers. Brain & Cognition 49, 170–181. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Boudreault, P., & Mayberry, R. L.
(2006) Grammatical processing in American Sign Language: Age of first-language acquisition effects in relation to syntactic structure. Language & Cognitive Processes 21(5), 608–635. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Boyes-Braem, P.
(1990) The acquisition of handshapes in American Sign Language. In V. Volterra, & C. Erting (Eds), From gesture to language in deaf and hearing children. New York, NY: Springer. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Caselli M. C., & Volterra, V. (1994) From communication to language in hearing and deaf children. In V. Volterra, & C. Erting (Eds), From gesture to language in hearing and deaf children (pp.263–278). New York, NY: Springer.Google Scholar
Caselli, N. K., & Pyers, J. E.
(2017) The road to language learning is not entirely iconic: Iconicity, neighborhood density, and frequency facilitate sign language acquisition. Psychological Science 28(7), 979–987. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Caselli, N. K., Sehyr, Z. S., Cohen-Goldberg A. M., & Emmorey, K.
(2017) ASL-LEX: A lexical database of American Sign Language. Behaviour Research Methods 49(2):784–801. Crossref.Google Scholar
Conlin, K. E., Mirus, G. R., Mauk, C., & Meier, R. P.
(2000) The acquisition of first signs: Place, handshape, and movement. In C. Chamberlain, J. P. Morford, & R. I. Mayberry (Eds.), Language acquisition by eye (pp.51–70). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Crowson, K.
(1994) Errors made by deaf children acquiring sign language. Early Child Development & Care 99, 63–78. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dingemanse, M., Blasi, D. E., Lupyan, G., Christiansen, M. H., & Monaghan, P.
(2015) Arbitrariness, iconicity, and systematicity in language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 19(10), 603–615. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dye, M. W. G., Hauser, P. C., & Bavelier, D.
(2009) Is visual attention in deaf individuals enhanced or deficient? The case of the Useful Field of View. PLoS One 4(5), e5640. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dye, M. W. G., & Hauser, P. C.
(2014) Sustained attention, selective attention and cognitive control in deaf and hearing children. Hearing Research 309, 94–102. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dye, M. W. G.
(2016) Response bias reveals enhanced attention to inferior visual field in signers of American Sign Language. Experimental Brain Research 234(4), 1067–1076. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Emmorey, K.
(2002) Language, cognition, and the brain: Insights from sign language research. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
(2000) The signs of language revisited: An anthology to honor Ursula Bellugi and Edward Klima. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Emmorey, K., Bosworth, R., & Kraljic, T.
(2009) Visual feedback and self-monitoring of sign language. Journal of Memory & Language 61(3), 398–411. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Emmorey K., Gertsberg, N., Korpics, F., & Wright, C. E.
(2009) The influence of visual feedback and register changes on sign language production: A kinematic study with deaf signers. Applied Psycholinguistics 30, 187–203. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Emmorey, K., Korpics, F., & Petronio, K.
(2009) The use of visual feedback during signing: Evidence from signers with impaired vision. The Journal of Deaf Studies & Deaf Education 14(1), 99–104. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Emmorey, K., Thompson, R. L., & Colvin, R.
(2009) Eye gaze during comprehension of American Sign Language by native and beginning signers. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 14(2), 237–243. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Feng, J., & Spence, I.
(2014) Upper visual field advantage in localizing a target among distractors. i-Perception 5, 97–100. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fenlon, J., Cormier, K., & Brentari, D.
(2017) The phonology of sign languages. In S. J. Hannahs, & A. Bosch (Eds.), The Routledge handbook of phonological theory. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Fenlon, J., Cormier, K., Rentelis, R., Schembri, A., Rowley, K., Adam, R., & Woll, B. (2014) BSL SignBank: A lexical database of British Sign Language (first ed.). London: Deafness, Cognition and Language Research Centre, University College London.Google Scholar
Frishberg, N.
(1975) Arbitrariness and iconicity: Historical change in American Sign Language. Language 51(3), 696–719. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gentner, D.
(2006) Why verbs are hard to learn. In K. A. Hirsh-Pasek & R. M. Golinkoff (Eds.), Action meets word: How children learn verbs (pp.544–564). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hillairet de Boisferon, A., Tift, A. H., Minar, N. J, & Lewkowicz, D. J.
(2015) Selective attention to a talker’s mouth in infancy: role of audiovisual temporal synchrony and linguistic experience. Developmental Science 20(3), e12381. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hoffman, M., Tiddens, E., Quittner, A. L. and the CDaCI Investigative Team
(2018) Comparisons of visual attention in school-age children with cochlear implants versus hearing peers and normative data. Hearing Research 359, 91–100. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hoffmeister, R., & Wilbur, R.
(1980) The acquisition of sign language. In H. Lane & F. Grosjean (Eds.), Recent perspectives on American Sign Language. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Hohenberger, A., Happ, D., & Leuniger, H.
(2002) Modality-dependent aspects of sign language production: Evidence from slips of the hands and their repairs in German Sign Language. In R. P. Meier, K. A. Cormier, & D. Quinto-Pozos (Eds.), Modality and structure in signed and spoken language (pp.112–142). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Höhle, B., Bijeljac-Babic, R., Herold, B., Weissenborn, J., & Nazzi, T.
(2009) Language specific prosodic preferences during the first year of life: Evidence from German and French infants. Infant Behavioural Development 32, 262–274. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kyle, J. G., & Woll, B.
(1985) Sign language: The study of deaf people and their language. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Levelt, P.
(1989) Speaking: From intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Lewkowicz, D. J., & Hansen-Tift, A. M.
(2012) Infants deploy selective attention to the mouth of a talking face when learning speech. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 109(5), 1431–1436. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lu, J., & Thompson, R. L.
(2017) Different word learning contexts modulate the learnability of iconic signs. Paper presented at the 8th Conference of the International Society for Gesture Studies – Gesture and Diversity. Cape Town.Google Scholar
Marentette, P. F., & Mayberry, R. I.
(2000) Principles for an emerging phonological system: A case study of early ASL acquisition. In C. Chamberlain, J. P. Morford, & R. I. Mayberry (Eds), Language Acquisition by Eye (pp.71–90). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Marschark, M., Siple, P., Lillo-Martin, D., Campbell, R., & Everhart, V. S.
(1997) Relations of language and thought: The view from sign language and deaf children. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Marschark, M., & Hauser, P. C.
(Eds.) (2008) Deaf cognition: foundations and outcomes. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mayberry, R. (2007) When timing is everything: Age of first-language acquisition effects on second-language learning. Applied Psycholinguistics 28(3), 537–549. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
McIntire, M. L.
(1977) The acquisition of American Sign Language hand configurations. Sign Language Studies 16(1), 247–266. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Meier, R.
(2006) The form of early signs: explaining signing children’s articulatory development. In B. Schick, M. Marschark, & P. E. Spencer (Eds), Advances in the sign language development of deaf children (pp.202–231). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Meier R. P., Cormier K., & Quinto-Pozos D.
(2002) Modality and structure in signed and spoken languages. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Meier, R. P., Mauk, C. E., Cheek, A., & Moreland, C. J.
(2008) The form of children’s early signs: Iconic or motoric determinants. Language Learning & Development 4(1), 63–98. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Morford, J. P.
(2003) Grammatical development in adolescent first language learners. Linguistics 41(4), 681–721. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Morford, J. P., & Carlson, M. L.
(2011) Sign perception and recognition in non-native signers of ASL. Language Learning & Development 7(2), 149–168. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Morgan, G., Barrett-Jones, S., & Stoneham, H.
(2007) The first signs of language: Phonological development in British Sign Language. Applied Psycholinguistics 28, 3–22. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Muir, L. J., & Richardson, I. E.
(2005) Perception of sign language and its application to visual communications for deaf people. Journal of Deaf Studies & Deaf Education 10(4), 390–401. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Newkirk, D., Klima, E., Pedersen, C. C., & Bellugi, U.
(1980) Linguistic evidence from slips of the hand. In V. Fromkin (Ed.), Errors in linguistic performance: slips of the tongue, ear, pen, and hand (pp.165–197). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Newport, E. L., & Meier, R. P.
(1985) The acquisition of American Sign Language. In D. I. Slobin (Ed.), The cross linguistic study of language acquisition, Volume 1: The data. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Ortega, G.
(2017) Iconicity and sign lexical acquisition: A review. Frontiers in Psychology 8, 1280. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ortega, G., & Morgan, G.
(2015) Phonological development in hearing learners of a sign language: The role of sign complexity and iconicity. Language Learning 65(3), 660–668. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2010) Comparing child and adult development of a visual phonological system. Language Interaction & Acquisition 1(1), 67–81. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ortega, G., Sumer, B., & Ozyurek, A.
(2017) Type of iconicity matters in the vocabulary development of signing children. Developmental Psychology 53(1), 89–99. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Perniss, P., Lu, J. C., Morgan, G., & Vigliocco, G.
(2018) Mapping language to the world: The role of iconicity in the sign language input. Developmental Science 21(2), e12551. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Perniss, P., Thompson, R. L., & Vigliocco, G. (2010) Iconicity as a general property of language: Evidence from spoken and signed languages. Frontiers in Psychology 1, 227. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Perry, L. K., Perlman, M., Winter, B., Massaro, D. W., & Lupyan, G.
(2018) Iconicity in the speech of children and adults. Developmental Science 21(3), e12572. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Petitto, L. A., & Marentette, P.
(1991) Babbling in the manual mode: Evidence for the ontogeny of language. Science 251, 1483–1496. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pizer, G., Meier, R. P., & Shaw Points, K.
(2011) Child-directed signing as a linguistic register. In R. Channon & H. van der Hulst (Eds.), Formational units in sign languages (pp.65–86). Berlin: Walter de Gruyter. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Reilly, J. S., McIntire, M. L., & Seago, H.
(1992) Affective prosody in American Sign Language. Sign Language Studies 75, 113–128. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ross, D. S., & Newport, E. L.
(1996) The development of language from non-native linguistic input. In A. Stringfellow, D. Cahana-Amitay, E. Hughs, & A. Zukowski (Eds.), Proceedings of the 20th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (volume 2, pp.634–645). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Sandler, W., & Lillo-Martin, D.
(2006) Sign language and linguistic universals. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schick, B.
(1990) The effects of morphosyntactic structure on the acquisition of classifier predicates in American Sign Language. In C. Lucas (Ed.), Sign language research: Theoretical issues. Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.Google Scholar
Schlenker, P.
(2018) Visible meaning: Sign language and the foundations of semantics. Theoretical Linguistics 44(3–4): 123–208.Google Scholar
Shield, A., & Meier, R. P.
(2018) Learning an embodied visual language: Four imitation strategies available to sign learners. Frontiers in Psychology 9, 811. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Siedlecki, T., & Bonvillian, J. D.
(1993) Location, handshape, and movement: Young children’s acquisition of the formational aspects of American Sign Language. Sign Language Studies 78, 31–52. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Singleton, J. L., & Newport, E. L.
(2004) When learners surpass their models: The acquisition of American Sign Language from inconsistent input. Cognitive Psychology 49(4), 370–407. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Siple, P.
(1978) Visual constraints for sign language communication. Sign Language Studies 19, 95–110. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Smith, L. B., Quittner, A. L., Osberger, M. J., & Miyamoto R.
(1998) Audition and visual attention: The developmental trajectory in deaf and hearing populations. Developmental Psychology 34(5), 840–850. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Steinberg, L.
(2005) Cognitive and affective development in adolescence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9(2), 69–74. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Stoll, C., & Dye, M. W. G.
(2019) Sign language experience redistributes attentional resources to the inferior visual field. Cognition 191, 103957. doi: CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Stone, A., Petitto L., & Bosworth, R.
(2018) Visual sonority modulates infants’ attraction to sign language. Language Learning & Development 14(2), 130–148. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Swisher, M. V., Christie, K., & Miller, S. L. (1989) The reception of signs in peripheral vision by deaf persons. Sign Language Studies 63, 99–125. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tharpe, A. M., Ashmead, D., Sladen, D. P., Ryan, H. A., & Rothpletz, A. M.
(2008) Visual attention and hearing loss: Past and current perspectives. Journal of the American Academy of Audiology 19(10), 741–747. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Thompson, R. L.
(2011) Iconicity in language processing and acquisition: What signed languages reveal. Language & Linguistics Compass 5, 603–616. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Thompson, R. L, Emmorey, K., & Gollan, T. H.
(2005) “Tip of the fingers” experiences by deaf signers. Psychological Science 16(11), 856–860. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Thompson, R. L., Vinson, D. P., & Vigliocco, G.
(2009) The link between form and meaning in American Sign Language: Lexical processing effects in a phonological decision task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Language, Memory, & Cognition 35(2), 550–557.Google Scholar
Thompson, R. L., Vinson, D. P., Woll, B., & Vigliocco, G.
(2012) The road to language learning is iconic: Evidence from British Sign Language. Psychological Science 23, 1443–1448. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Vinson, D. P., Thompson, R. L., Skinner, R., & Vigliocco, G.
(2015) A faster path between meaning and form? Iconicity facilitates sign recognition and production in British Sign Language. Journal of Memory & Language 82, 56–85. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Vinson, D. P., Thompson, R. L., Skinner, R., Fox, N., & Vigliocco, G.
(2010) The hands and mouth do not always slip together in British Sign Language: Dissociating articulatory channels in the lexicon. Psychological Science 21(8), 1158–1167. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Watkins, F., & Thompson, R. L.
(2017) The relationship between sign production and sign comprehension: What handedness reveals. Cognition 164, 144–149. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Waxman, S., Fu, X., Arunachalam, S., Leddon, E., Geraghty, K., & Song, H. J.
(2013) Are nouns learned before verbs? Infants provide insight into a longstanding debate. Child Development Perspectives 7(3), 155–159. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Werker, J. F., & Tees, R. C.
(1984) Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behavioural Development 7, 49–63. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Woll, B.
(1987) Historical and comparative aspects of BSL. In J. G. Kyle (Ed.), Sign and school (pp.12–34). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Woodward, J. C.
(1976) Signs of change: historical variation in American Sign Language. Sign Language Studies 10, 81–94. CrossrefGoogle Scholar