Part of
Current Perspectives on Child Language Acquisition: How children use their environment to learn
Edited by Caroline F. Rowland, Anna L. Theakston, Ben Ambridge and Katherine E. Twomey
[Trends in Language Acquisition Research 27] 2020
► pp. 287321
References
Aldred, C., Green, J., Emsley, R., & McConachie, H.
(2012) Brief report: Mediation of treatment effect in a communication intervention for pre-school children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42, 447–454. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Aldridge, M., Stone, K., Sweeney, M., & Bower, T.
(2000) Preverbal children with autism understand the intentions of others. Developmental Science, 3, 294. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
American Psychiatric Association
(2013) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bannard, C., & Matthews, D.
(2008) Stored word sequences in language learning: The effect of familiarity on children’s repetition of four-word combinations. Psychological Science, 19(3), 241–248. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bani Hani, H., Gonzalez-Barrero, A., & Nadig, A.
(2013) Children’s referential understanding of novel words and parent labelling behaviours: similarities across children with and without autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Language, 40(5), 971–1002. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bang, J., & Nadig, A.
(2015) Learning language in autism: Maternal linguistic input contributes to later vocabulary. Autism Research, 8, 214–223. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Baldwin, D., & Crowson, M.
(1997) Do children with autism use the speaker’s direction of gaze strategy to crack the code of language? Child Development, 68(1), 48–57. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U.
(1985) Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21(1), 37–46. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bates, E., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V.
(1975) The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 21, 205–226.Google Scholar
Bauminger-Zviely, N., Eden, S., Zancanaro, M., Weiss, P., & Gal, E.
(2013) Increasing social engagement in children with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder using collaborative technologies in the school environment. Autism, 17, 317–339. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bauminger-Zviely, N., Karin, E., Kimhi, Y., & Agam-Ben-Artzi, G.
(2014) Spontaneous peer conversation in preschoolers with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder versus typical development. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 55(4), 363–373. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bettelheim, B.
(1967) The empty fortress: Infantile autism and the birth of the self. New York, NY: The Free Press.Google Scholar
Bishop, D. V., Snowling, M. J., Thompson, P. A., & Greenhalgh, T.
(2016) CATALISE: A multinational and multidisciplinary delphi consensus study of problems with language development. Phase 2: Terminology. PeerJ Preprints, 4, e2484v1.Google Scholar
Bishop, D. V., Whitehouse, A. J., Watt, H. J., & Line, E. A.
(2008) Autism and diagnostic substitution: Evidence from a study of adults with a history of developmental language disorder. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 50, 341–345. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bottema-Beutel, K., Kim, S., & Crowley, S.
(2019) A systematic review and meta-regression analysis of social functioning in autism and typical development. Autism Research, 12(2), 152–175. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boucher, J.
(2012) Research review: Structural language in autistic spectrum disorder – Characteristics and causes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53(3), 219–233. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brandone, C. A., Pence, K. L., Golinkoff, R. M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K.
(2007) Action speaks louder than words: Young children differentially weight perceptual, social, and linguistic cues to learn verbs. Child Development, 78, 1322–1342. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bratman, M.
(1992) Shared cooperative activity. Philosophical Review, 101(2), 327–340. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brownell, C.
(2011) Early developments in joint action. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 2(2), 193–211. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J.
(1974) From communication to language – A psychological perspective. Cognition, 3(3), 255–287. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bruinsma, Y., Koegel, R., & Koegel, L.
(2004) Joint attention and children with autism: A review of the literature. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 10, 169–175. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Calder, L., Hill, V., & Pellicano, E.
(2013) ‘Sometimes I want to play by myself’: Understanding what friendship means to children with autism in mainstream primary schools. Autism, 17, 296–316. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cameron-Faulkner, T., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M.
(2003) A construction-based analysis of child-directed speech. Cognitive Science, 27, 843–873. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cameron-Faulkner, T., Theakston, A., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M.
(2015) The relationship between infant holdouts and gives and pointing. Infancy, 20(5), 576–586. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, M., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J.
(2001) Understanding of others intentions in children with autism and children with developmental delays. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(6), 589–599. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2002) Interrelations among social-cognitive skills in young children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32(2), 91–106. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., Baird, G., Drew, A., & Cox, A.
(2003) Predicting language outcome in infants with autism and pervasive developmental disorder. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 38(3), 265–285. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Charman, T., Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Cox, A., Baird, G. & Drew, A.
(1997) ‘Infants with Autism: An Investigation of Empathy, Pretend Play, Joint Attention, and Imitation’, Developmental Psychology 33: 781–9. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cherkàssky, V., Rajesh, K., Keller, T., & Just, A.
(2016) Functional connectivity in a baseline resting-state network in autism. Neuroreport, 17, 1687–1690. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chevallier, C., Kohls, G., Troiani, V., Brodkin, E., & Schultz, R.
(2012) The social motivation theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Science, 16(4), 231–239. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N.
(2011) Language and other cognitive systems. What is special about language? Language Learning and Development, 7, 263–278. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, H. H.
(1996) Using language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clifford, S., & Dissanayake, C.
(2008) The early development of joint attention in infants with autistic disorder using home video observations and parental interview. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 791–805. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Colombi, C., Liebal, K., Tomasello, M., Young, G., Warneken, F., & Rogers, S.
(2009) Examining correlates of cooperation in autism. Autism, 13(2), 143–163. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Conti-Ramsden, G., Simkin, Z., & Botting, N.
(2006) The prevalence of autistic spectrum disorders in adolescents with a history of specific language impairment (SLI). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 621–628. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crain, S., Thornton, R., & Murasugi, K.
(2009) Capturing the evasive passive. Language Acquisition, 16, 123–133. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Curtis, P., Frey, J., Watson, C., Hampton, L., & Roberts, M.
(2018) Language disorders and problem behaviours: A meta-analysis. Pediatrics, 142(2), e20173551. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
D’Entremont, B.
(2000) A perceptual-attentional explanation of gaze following in 3- and 6-month-olds. Developmental Science, 3, 302–311. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Department for Education and Skills (DfES)
(2002) Autism spectrum disorders: Good practice guidance. Part 1. London: DfES.Google Scholar
Dittmar, M., Abbot-Smith, K., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M.
(2014) Familiar verbs are not always easier than novel verbs: How German pre-school children comprehend active and passive sentences. Cognitive Science, 38, 128–151. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ebert, K., & Kohnert, K.
(2011) Sustained attention in children with primary language impairment: A meta-analysis. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 54(5), 1372–1384. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Evans, J. L., Saffran, J. R., & Robe-Torres, K.
(2009) Statistical learningin children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52(2), 321–335. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Foti, F., de Crescenzo, F., Vivanti, G., Menghini, D., & Vicari, S.
(2015) Implicit learning in individuals with autism spectrum disorders: A meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 45, 897–910. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Freudenthal, D., Pine, J. M., Jones, G., & Gobet, F.
(2015) Simulating the cross-linguistic pattern of Optional Infinitive errors in children’s declaratives and Wh- questions. Cognition, 143, 61–76. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fusaroli, R., Weed, E., Fein, D., & Naigles, L.
(2019) Hearing me hearing you: Reciprocal effects between child and parent language in autism and typical development. Cognition, 183, 1–18. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gabriel, A., Maillart, C., Stefaniak, N., Lejuene, C., Desmottes, L., & Meulemans, T.
(2013) Procedural learning in specific language impairment: Effects of sequence complexity. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 19(3), 264–271. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gallinat, E., & Spaulding, T.
(2014) Differences in the performance of children with specific language impairment and their typically developing peers on nonverbal cognitive tests: A meta-analysis. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 57, 1363–1382. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gillespie-Lynch, K., Khalulyan, A., del Rosario, M., McCarthy, B., Gomez, L., Sigman, M., & Hutman, T.
(2015) Is early joint attention associated with school-age pragmatic language? Autism, 19(2), 168–177. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, A.
(1999) The emergence of the semantics of argument structure constructions. In B. MacWhinney (Ed.), The emergence of language (pp. 197–212). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Gonzalez-Barrero, A., & Nadig, A.
(2018) Bilingual children with autism spectrum disorders: The impact of amount of language exposure on vocabulary and morphological skills at school age. Autism Research, 11, 1667–1678. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gräfenhain, M., Behne, T., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M.
(2009) Young children’s understanding of joint commitments. Developmental Psychology, 45(5), 1430–1443. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Greenslade, K., Utter, E., & Landa, R.
(2019) Predictors of pragmatic communication in school-age siblings of children with ASD and low-risk controls. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 49(4), 1352–1365. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hale, C. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H.
(2005) Social communication in children with autism. Autism, 9(2), 157–178. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hallmayer, J., Cleveland, S., Torres, A., Phillips, J., Cohen, B., Torigoe, T., Miller, J., Fedele, A., Collins, J., Smith, K., Lotspeich, L., Coren, L., Ozonoff, S., Lajonchere, C., Grether, J., & Risch, N.
(2011) Genetic heritability and shared environmental factors among twin pairs with autism. Archives of Genetic Psychiatry, 68, 1095–1102. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hamann, K., Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M.
(2012) Children’s developing commitments to joint goals. Child Development, 83(1), 137–145. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hamilton, A.
(2009) Research review: Goals, intentions and mental states: Challenges for theories of autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50(8), 881–892. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hamilton, A., Brindley, R., & Frith, U.
(2007) Imitation and action understanding in autistic spectrum disorders: How valid is the hypothesis of a deficit in the mirror neuron system? Neuropsychologia, 45, 1859–1868. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Happé, F.
(2015) Autism as a neurodevelopmental disorder of mind-reading. Journal of the British Academy, 3, 197–209. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hedenius, H., Persson, J., Tremblay, A., Adi-Japha, E., Verissimo, J., Dye, C. D., Alm, P., Jennische, M., Tomblin, B., & Ullman, M.
(2011) Grammar predicts procedural learning and consolidation deficits in children with specific language impairment. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32(6), 2362–2375. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Herbert, M., Ziegler, D., Deutch, C., O’Brien, L., Kennedy, D., Filipek, P., Bakardjiev, A., Hodgson, J., Takeoka, M., Makris, N., & Caviness, V.
(2005) Brain asymmetries in autism and developmental language disorder: a nested whole-brain analysis. Brain, 128, 213–226. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hill, E.
(2001) Non-specific nature of specific language impairment: A review of the literature with regard to concomitant motor impairments. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 36(2), 149–171. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hobson, R. P.
(1993) Autism and the development of mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Huang, C-T., Chiang, C-H., & Hung, C-Y.
(2017) Young children with autism imitate in the context of others’ prior intention. Autism, 21(1), 83–91. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hurtado, N., Marchman, V., & Fernald, A.
(2008) Does input influence uptake? Links between maternal talk, processing speed and vocabulary size in Spanish-learning children. Developmental Science, 11, F31–F39. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jaswal, V. & Akhtar, N.
2019Being versus appearing socially uninterested: challenging assumptions about social motivation in autism. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 42:e82 DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jones, J., Simonoff, E., Baird, G., Pickles, A., Marsden, A., Tregay, J., Happé, F., & Charman, T.
(2018) The association between Theory of Mind, executive function and the symptoms of Autism Spectrum Disorder. Autism Research, 11, 95–109. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Just, M., Cherkassky, V., Keller, T., Kana, R., & Minshew, N.
(2007) Functional and anatomical cortical underconnectivity in autism: Evidence from an fMRI study of an exec function task and corpus callosum morphometry. Cerebral Cortex, 17, 951–961. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kaartinen, M., Puura, K., Pispa, P., Helminen, M., Salmelin, R., Pelkonen, E., Juujarvi, P., Kessler, E., & Skuse, D.
(2019) Associations between cooperation, reactive aggression and social impairments among boys with autism spectrum disorder. Autism, 23(1), 154–166. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kidd, E., & Arciuli, J.
(2016) Individual differences in statistical learning predict children’s comprehension of syntax. Child Development, 87(1), 184–193. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kidd, E., Donnelly, S. & Christiansen, M.
(2018) Individual differences in language acquisition and processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(2): 154–169. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kimhi, Y., & Bauminger-Zviely, N.
(2012) Collaborative problem solving in young typical development and HFASD. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42, 1984–1997. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kirjavainen, M., Theakston, A., & Lieven, E.
(2009) Can input explain children’s me-for-I errors? Journal of Child Language, 36, 1091–1114. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., & Volkmar, F.
(2003) The enactive mind, or from actions to cognition: lessons from autism. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 358, 345–360. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kloo, D., & Perner, J.
(2003) Training transfer between card sorting and false belief understanding: Helping children apply conflicting descriptions. Child Development, 74(6): 1823–1839. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Krisnan, S., & Watkins, K.
(2019) A challenge for the procedural deficit hypothesis: How should we measure sequential learning in childhood. Developmental Science, 22(4), e12815. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lai, C. L. E., Lau, Z., Lui, S. S., Lok, E., Tam, V., Chan, Q., Cheng, K. M., Lam, S. M., & Cheung, E. F. C.
(2017) Meta-analysis of neuropsychological measures of executive functioning in children and adolescents with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research, 10(5), 911–939. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Landa, R.
(2005) Assessment of social communication skills in preschoolers. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 11, 247–252. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Landa, R., Holman, K., & Garrett-Mayer, E.
(2007) Social and communication development in toddlers with early and later diagnoses of autism spectrum disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64, 853–864. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Landa, R., Gross, A., Stuart, E., Bauman, M.
(2012) Latent class analysis of early developmental trajectory in baby siblings of children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53(9), 986–996. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leekam, S., Hunnisett, E., & Moore, C.
(1998) Targets and cues: Gaze-following in children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39(7), 951–962. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leekam, S., Prior, M., & Uljarevic, M.
(2011) Restricted and repetitive behaviors in Autism Spectrum Disorders: A review of research in the last decade. Psychological Bulletin, 137(4), 562–593. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leonard, L.
(2000) Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Leonard, L., Sabbadini, L., Leonard, J., & Volterra, V.
(1987) Specific language impairment in children: A cross-linguistic study. Brain and Language, 32(2), 233–252. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Li, J., Zhu, L., Liu, J., & Li, X.
(2014) Social and non-social deficits in children with high-functioning autism and their cooperative behaviors. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 8, 1657–1671. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liebal, K., Behne, T., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M.
(2009) Infants use shared experience to interpret pointing gestures. Developmental Science, 12(2), 264–271. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liebal, K., Colombi, C., Rogers, S. J., Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M.
(2008) Helping and cooperation in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(2), 224–238. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lieven, E.
(1994) Crosslinguistic and crosscultural aspects of language addressed to children. In C. Gallaway & B. Richards (Eds.), Input and interaction in language acquisition (pp. 56–73). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2010) Input and first language acquisition: Evaluating the role of frequency. Lingua, 120, 2546–2556. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2016) Usage-based approaches to language development: Where do we go from here? Language and Cognition, 8, 346–368. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lieven, E., Behrens, H., Speares, J., & Tomasello, M.
(2003) Early syntactic creativity: A usage-based approach. Journal of Child Language, 30, 333–370. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lieven, E., Pine, J., & Baldwin, G.
(1997) Lexically-based learning and early grammatical development. Journal of Child Language, 24, 187–219. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lind, S. E., Williams, D. M., Bowler, D. M., & Peel, A.
(2014) Episodic memory and episodic future thinking impairments in high-functioning autism spectrum disorder: An underlying difficulty with scene construction or self-projection? Neuropsychology, 28(1), 55–67. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lindner, K., & Johnson, J.
(1992) Grammatical morphology in language-impaired children acquiring English or German as their first language: A functional perspective. Applied Psycholinguistics, 13, 115–129. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., Henning, A., Striano, T., & Tomasello, M.
(2004) Twelve month olds point to share attention and interest. Developmental Science, 7(3), 297–307. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liu, X.-Q., Peterson, A., & Szatmari, P.
(2008) Genome-wide linkage analyses of quantitative and categorical autism subphenotypes. Biological Psychiatry, 64, 561–570. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Locke, J., Williams, J., Shih, W. & Kasari, C.
(2017) Characteristics of socially successful elementary school-aged children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58(1), 94–102. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lord, C., Rutter, M., Dilavore, P., Risi, S., Gotham, K., & Bishop, S.
(2012) Autism diagnostic observation schedule (2nd ed., ADOS-2, Modules 1–4). Torrance, CA: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
Loucas, T., Charman, T., Pickles, A., Simonoff, E., Chandler, S., Meldrum, D., & Baird, G.
(2008) Autistic symptomatology and language ability in autism spectrum disorder and specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49(11), 1184–1192. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lum, J., Conti-Ramsden, G., Page, D., & Ullman, M.
(2012) Working, declarative and procedural memory in specific language impairment. Cortex, 48(9), 1138–1154. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Luyster, R. & Lord, L.
(2009) Word learning in children with autism spectrum disorders. Developmental Psychology, 45, 1774–1786. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Luyster, R., & Arunachalam, S.
(2018) Brief report: Learning language through overhearing in children with ASD. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Macari, S., Campbell, D., Gengoux, G., Saulnier, C., Klin, A., & Chawarska, K.
(2012) Predicting developmental status from 12 to 24 months in infants at risk for autism spectrum disorder: A preliminary report. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42, 2636–2647. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Malkin, L., Abbot-Smith, K., & Williams, D.
(2018) Usage and interpretation of verbal reference by children and adults with Autism Spectrum Disorder: A systematic review Autism and Developmental Language Impairments, 3, 1–24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Malkin, L., Abbot-Smith, K., Williams, D., & Ayling, J.
(2018) When do children with Autism Spectrum Disorder take common ground into account during communication? Autism Research, 11, 1366–1375. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Matthews, D., & Bannard, C.
(2010) Children’s production of unfamiliar word sequences is predicted by positional variability and latent classes in a large sample of child-directed speech. Cognitive Science, 34(3), 465–488. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Matthews, D., Lieven, E., Theakston, A., & Tomasello, M.
(2005) The role of frequency in the acquisition of English word order. Cognitive Development, 20, 121–136. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Maye, J., Werker, J. F., & Gerken, L.
(2002) Infant sensitivity to distributional information can affect phonetic discrimination. Cognition, 82, B101–B111. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McGillon, M., Pine, J., Herbert, J., & Matthews, D.
(2017) A randomised controlled trial to test the effect of promoting caregiver contingent talk on language development in infants from diverse SES backgrounds. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58(10), 1122–1131. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McDuffie, A., Yoder, P., & Stone, W.
(2005) Prelinguistic predictors of vocabulary in young children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 48, 1080–1097. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Melis, A., Hare, B., & Tomasello, M.
(2006) Chimpanzees recruit the best collaborators. Science, 311, 1297–1300. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mundy, P., Sigman, M., Ungerer, J., & Sherman, T.
(1986) Defining the social deficits of autism: the contribution of nonverbal communication measures. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27, 657–669. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mundy, P., Sullivan, L., & Mastergeorge, A.
(2009) A parallel and distributed processing model of joint attention, social-cognition and autism. Autism Research, 2(1), 2–1. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Murphy, S., Faulkner, D., & Farley, L.
(2014) The behaviour of young children with social communication disorders during dyadic interaction with peers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 42(2), 277–289. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Murza, K., Schwartz, J., Hahs-Vaghn, D., & Nye, C.
(2016) Joint attention interventions for children with autism spectrum disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 51(3), 236–251. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nash, J.
(1951) Non-cooperative games. The Annuals of Mathematics, 54, 286–295. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Norbury, C., Griffiths, H., & Nation, K.
(2010) Sound before meaning: Word learning in autistic disorders. Neuropsychologia, 48, 4012–4019. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Obeid, R., Brooks, B., Powers, K., Gillespie-Lynch, K., & Lum, J.
(2016) Statistical learning in specific language impairment and autism spectrum disorder: A meta-analysis. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1245.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Oetting, J., & Rice, M.
(1993) Plural acquisition in children with Specific Language Impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 1236–1248. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Over, H.
(2016) The origins of belonging: Social motivation in infants and young children. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 371. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Owens, G., Granader, Y., Humphrey, A., & Baron-Cohen, S.
(2008) LEGO® therapy and the social use of language programme: An evaluation of two social skills interventions for children with high functioning autism and Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(10), 1944–1957. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Parish-Morris, J., Hennon, E., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R., & Tager-Flusberg, H.
(2007) Children with autism illuminate the role of social intention in word learning. Child Development, 78, 1265–1287. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Park, J., Miller, C., Rosenbaum, D., Sanjeevan, T., van Hell, J., Weiss, D., & Mainela-Arnold, E.
(2018) Bilingualism and procedural learning in typically-developing and children with language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 61, 634–644. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paul, R.
(2008) Interventions to improve communication. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 17(4), 835–856. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paul, R., Orlovski, S., Marcinko, H., & Volkmar, F.
(2009) Conversational behaviors in youth with high-functioning ASD and Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(1): 115. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pine, J., Lieven, E., & Rowland, C.
(1998) Comparing different models of the English verb category. Linguistics, 36, 807–830. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pine, J., Freudenthal, D., Krajewski, G., & Gobet, F.
(2013) Do young children have adult-like syntactic categories? Zipf’s law and the case of the determiner. Cognition, 127(3): 345–360. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S.
(1989) Learnability and cognition. The acquisition of argument structure. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
(1999) Words and rules. London: Weidenfeld & Nicolson.Google Scholar
Plunkett, K., & Juola, P.
(1999) A connectionist model of English past tense and plural morphology. Cognitive Science, 23(4), 463–490. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Preissler, M., & Carey, S.
(2005) The role of inferences about referential intent in word learning: evidence from autism. Cognition, 97, B13–B23. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roser, M., Aslin, R., McKenzie, R., Zahra, D., & Fiser, J.
(2015) Enhanced visual statistical learning in adults with autism. Neuropsychology, 29(2), 163–172. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rowland, C., Pine, J., Lieven, E., & Theakston, A.
(2005) The incidence of error in young children’s Wh-questions. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 48, 384–404. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roy, P., & Chiat, S.
(2014) Developmental pathways of language and social communication problems in 9–11 year olds: Unpicking the heterogeneity. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 35(10), 2534–2546. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rozga, A., Hutman, T., Young, G., Rogers, S., Ozonoff, S., Dapretto, M., & Sigman, M.
(2011) Behavioural profiles of affected and unaffected siblings of children with autism: contribution of measures of mother–infant interaction and non-verbal communication. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41, 287–301. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Saffran, J. R., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L.
(1996) Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science, 274, 1926–1928. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sally, D., & Hill, E.
(2006) The development of interpersonal strategy: Autism, theory-of-mind, cooperation and fairness. Journal of Economic Psychology, 27(1), 73–97. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sandbank, M., & Yoder, P.
(2016) The association between parental mean length of utterance and language outcomes in children with disabilities: A correlational meta-analysis. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 25(2), 240–251. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Searle, J.
(1990) Collective intentions and actions. In P. Cohen, J. Morgan, & M. Pollack (Eds.), Intentions in communication (pp. 401–415). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Sengottuvel, K., & Rao, P.
(2013) Aspects of grammar sensitive to procedural memory deficits in children with specific language impairment. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34(10), 3317–3331. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sigman, M., & Ruskin, E.
(1999) Continuity and change in the social competence of children with autism, Down syndrome and developmental delays. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 64(1), 1–114. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Simkin, Z., & Conti-Ramsden, G.
(2001) Nonword repetition and grammatical morphology: Normative data for children in their final year of primary school. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 36, 395–404. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Siposova, B., & Carpenter, M.
(2019) A new look at joint attention and common knowledge. Cognition, 189, 260–274. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sng, C., Carter, M., & Stephenson, J.
(2018) A systematic review of the comparative pragmatic differences in conversational skills of individuals with autism. Autism and Developmental Language Impairments, 3, 1–24.Google Scholar
Tayama, M., Tateno, M., Park, T., Ukai, W., Hashimoto, E., & Saito, T.
(2012) The study of cognitive characteristics in asperger’s disorder by using a modified prisoner’s dilemma game with variable payoff matrix. PloS One, 7(11), e48794. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Thomas, M., & Karmiloff-Smith, A.
(2003) Modelling language acquisition in atypical phenotypes. Psychological Review, 110(4), 647–682. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tick, B., Bolton, P., Happé, F., Rutter, M., & Rijsdijk, F.
(2016) Heritability of autism spectrum disorders: A meta-analysis of twin studies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 57(5), 585–595. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M.
(2003) Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
(2008) Origins of human communication. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2018) How children come to understand false beliefs: A shared intentionality account. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(34), 8491–8498. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H.
(2005) Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 675–735. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Hamann, K.
(2012) Collaboration in young children. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(1), 1–12. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Moll, H.
(2010) The gap is social: Human shared intentionality and culture. In P. Kappeler & J. Silk (Eds.), Mind the gap: Tracing the origins of human universals (pp. 331–349). Berlin: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomblin, B., Mainela-Arnold, E., & Zhang, X.
(2007) Procedural learning in adolescents with and without Specific Language Impairment. Language Learning and Development, 3(4), 269–293. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tuomela, R.
(2007) The philosophy of sociality. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ullman, M., & Pierpont, E.
(2005) Specific language impairment is not specific to language: The procedural deficit hypothesis. Cortex, 41(3), 399–433. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Valian, V. V.
(2009) Innateness and learnability. In E. Bavin (Ed.), Handbook of child language (pp.15–34). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van der Lely, H., & Ullman, M.
(2001) Past tense morphology in specifically language impaired and normally developing children. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16, 177–217. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Ommeren, T., Begeer, S., Scheeren, A., & Koot, H.
(2012) Measuring reciprocity in high functioning children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42, 1001–1010. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Venker, C., Bolt, D., Meyer, A., Sindberg, H., Weismer, S., & Tager-Flusberg, H.
(2015) Parent telegraphic speech use and spoken language in pre-schoolers in ASD. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 56(6), 1733–1746. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
West, G., Vadillo, M., Shanks, D., & Hulme, C.
(2017) The procedural learning deficit hypothesis of language learning disorders: We see some problems. Developmental Science, 21(2), e12552. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Williams, D., & Happé, F.
(2010) Representing intentions in self and others: Studies of autism and typical development. Developmental Science, 13, 307–319. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wing, L., & Gould, J.
(1979) Severe impairments of social interaction and associated abnormalities in children: Epidemiology and classification. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 9, 11–29.Google Scholar
Wonnacott, E.
(2011) Balancing generalization and lexical conservatism: An artificial language study with child learners. Journal of Memory and Language, 65, 1–14. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yew, S., & O’Kearney, R.
(2013) Emotional and behavioural outcomes later in childhood and adolescence for children with specific language impairments: Meta-analysis o controlled prospective studies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(5), 516–524. DOI logoGoogle Scholar