References
Allen, M. L., Haywood, S., Rajendran, G., & Branigan, H.
(2011) Evidence for syntactic alignment in children with autism: Syntactic alignment in autism. Developmental Science, 14 (3), 540–548. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Arai, M., van Gompel, R. P. G., & Scheepers, C.
(2007) Priming ditransitive structures in comprehension. Cognitive Psychology, 54 (3), 218–250. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J.
(2013) Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68 , 255–278. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bencini, G. M. L., & Valian, V. V.
(2008) Abstract sentence representations in 3-year-olds: Evidence from language production and comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 59 (1), 97–113. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bidgood, A., Pine, J. M., Rowland, C. F., & Ambridge, B.
(2020) Syntactic representations are both abstract and semantically constrained: Evidence from children’s and adults’ comprehension and production/priming of the English passive. Cognitive Science, 44 (9), e12892. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bock, J. K.
(1986) Syntactic persistence in language production. Cognitive Psychology, 18 , 355–387. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bock, K., & Griffin, Z. M.
(2000) The persistence of structural priming: Transient activation or implicit learning? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129 (2), 177–192. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Boudewyn, M. A., Zirnstein, M., Swaab, T. Y., & Traxler, M. J.
(2014) Priming prepositional phrase attachment: Evidence from eye-tracking and event-related potentials. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67 (3), 424–454. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Brandt, S., Nitschke, S., & Kidd, E.
(2017) Priming the comprehension of German object relative clauses. Language Learning and Development, 13 (3), 241–261. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Branigan, H. P., & McLean, J. F.
(2016) What children learn from adults’ utterances: An ephemeral lexical boost and persistent syntactic priming in adult–child dialogue. Journal of Memory and Language, 91 , 141–157. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Branigan, H. P., McLean, J. F., & Jones, M.
(2005a) A blue cat or a cat that is blue? Evidence for abstract syntax in young children’s noun phrases. In A. Brugos, M. R. Clark-Cotton, & S. Ha (Eds.), BUCLD29:Proceedings of the 29th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (Vol. 1, pp. 109–121). Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Branigan, H. P., & Messenger, K.
(2016) Consistent and cumulative effects of syntactic experience in children’s sentence production: Evidence for error-based implicit learning. Cognition, 157 , 250–256. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Branigan, H. P., Pickering, M. J., & Cleland, A. A.
(2000) Syntactic co-ordination in dialogue. Cognition, 75 (2), B13–B25. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Branigan, H. P., Pickering, M. J., & McLean, J. F.
(2005b) Priming prepositional-phrase attachment during comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31 (3), 468–481. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Buckle, L., Lieven, E., & Theakston, A. L.
(2017) The effects of animacy and syntax on priming: A developmental study. Frontiers in Psychology, 8 , 2246. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chang, F., Dell, G. S., & Bock, K.
(2006) Becoming syntactic. Psychological Review, 113 (2), 234–272. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chang, F., Dell, G. S., Bock, K., & Griffin, Z. M.
(2000) Structural priming as implicit learning: A comparison of models of sentence production. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 29 (2), 217–229. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cleland, A. A., & Pickering, M. J.
(2003) The use of lexical and syntactic information in language production: Evidence from the priming of noun-phrase structure. Journal of Memory and Language, 49 (2), 214–230. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fazekas, J., Jessop, A., Pine, J., & Rowland, C.
(2020) Do children learn from their prediction mistakes? A registered report evaluating error-based theories of language acquisition. Royal Society Open Science, 7 (11), 180877. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Foltz, A., Thiele, K., Kahsnitz, D., & Stenneken, P.
(2015) Children’s syntactic-priming magnitude: Lexical factors and participant characteristics. Journal of Child Language, 42 (4), 932–945. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gámez, P. B., & Shimpi, P. M.
(2016) Structural priming in Spanish as evidence of implicit learning. Journal of Child Language, 43 (1), 207–233. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gámez, P. B., Shimpi, P. M., Waterfall, H. R., & Huttenlocher, J.
(2009) Priming a perspective in Spanish monolingual children: The use of syntactic alternatives. Journal of Child Language, 36 (2), 269–290. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gámez, P. B., & Vasilyeva, M.
(2015) Exploring interactions between semantic and syntactic processes: The role of animacy in syntactic priming. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 138 , 15–30. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Garraffa, M., Coco, M. I., & Branigan, H. P.
(2015) Effects of immediate and cumulative syntactic experience in language impairment: Evidence from priming of subject relatives in children with SLI. Language Learning and Development, 11 (1), 18–40. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Goldwater, M. B., Tomlinson, M. T., Echols, C. H., & Love, B. C.
(2011) Structural priming as structure-mapping: Children use analogies from previous utterances to guide sentence production. Cognitive Science, 35 (1), 156–170. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hartsuiker, R. J., Bernolet, S., Schoonbaert, S., Speybroeck, S., & Vanderelst, D.
(2008) Syntactic priming persists while the lexical boost decays: Evidence from written and spoken dialogue. Journal of Memory and Language, 58 (2), 214–238. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Havron, N., Babineau, M., & Christophe, A.
(2021) 18-month-olds fail to use recent experience to infer the syntactic category of novel words. Developmental Science, 24 (2). CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Havron, N., Babineau, M., Fievet, A.-C., de Carvalho, A., & Christophe, A.
(2021) Syntactic prediction adaptation accounts for language processing and language learning. Language Learning, 71 (4), 1194–1221. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Havron, N., Carvalho, A., Fiévet, A., & Christophe, A.
(2019) Three- to four-year-old children rapidly adapt their predictions and use them to learn novel word meanings. Child Development, 90 (1), 82–90. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Havron, N., Scaff, C., Carbajal, M. J., Linzen, T., Barrault, A., & Christophe, A.
(2020) Priming syntactic ambiguity resolution in children and adults. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 35 (10), 1445–1455. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hesketh, A., Serratrice, L., & Ashworth, R.
(2016) Encouraging use of subordination in children’s narratives: A classroom-based priming study. Language Learning and Development, 12 (4), 413–428. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hopkins, Z., Yuill, N., & Keller, B.
(2016) Children with autism align syntax in natural conversation. Applied Psycholinguistics, 37 (2), 347–370. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Horgan, D.
(1978) The development of the full passive. Journal of Child Language, 5 (1), 65–80. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hsu, D.-B.
(2014a) Mandarin-speaking three-year-olds’ demonstration of productive knowledge of syntax: Evidence from syntactic productivity and structural priming with the SVO-ba alternation. Journal of Child Language, 41 (5), 1115–1146. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2014b) Structural priming as learning: Evidence from Mandarin-learning 5-year-olds. Language Acquisition, 21 (2), 156–172. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2019) Children’s adaption to input change using an abstract syntactic representation: Evidence from structural priming in Mandarin-speaking preschoolers. Frontiers in Psychology, 10 , 2186. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M., & Shimpi, P.
(2004) Syntactic priming in young children. Journal of Memory and Language, 50 (2), 182–195. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hutton, J., & Kidd, E.
(2011) Structural priming in comprehension of relative clause sentences: In search of a frequency x regularity interaction. In E. Kidd (Ed.), The acquisition of relative clauses: Processing, typology and function (pp. 227–242). John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jaeger, T. F., & Snider, N. E.
(2013) Alignment as a consequence of expectation adaptation: Syntactic priming is affected by the prime’s prediction error given both prior and recent experience. Cognition, 127 (1), 57–83. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kaschak, M. P.
(2007) Long-term structural priming affects subsequent patterns of language production. Memory & Cognition, 35 (5), 925–937. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kaschak, M. P., Kutta, T. J., & Coyle, J. M.
(2014) Long and short term cumulative structural priming effects. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 29 (6), 728–743. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kaschak, M. P., Loney, R. A., & Borreggine, K. L.
(2006) Recent experience affects the strength of structural priming. Cognition, 99 (3), B73–B82. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kidd, E.
(2012a) Implicit statistical learning is directly associated with the acquisition of syntax. Developmental Psychology, 48 (1), 171–184. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2012b) Individual differences in syntactic priming in language acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 33 (2), 393–418. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
LeDoux, K., Traxler, M. J., & Swaab, T. Y.
(2007) Syntactic priming in comprehension: Evidence from event-related potentials. Psychological Science, 18 (2), 135–143. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Manetti, C.
(2013) On the production of passives in Italian: Evidence from an elicited production task and a syntactic priming study with preschool children. In S. Baiz, N. Goldman, & R. Hawkes (Eds.), Proceedings of the 37th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development online supplement (pp. 1–16). Boston University.Google Scholar
Messenger, K.
(2021) The persistence of priming: Exploring long-lasting syntactic priming effects in children and adults. Cognitive Science, 45 (6). CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Messenger, K., Branigan, H. P., & McLean, J. F.
(2011) Evidence for (shared) abstract structure underlying children’s short and full passives. Cognition, 121 (2), 268–274. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2012a) Is children’s acquisition of the passive a staged process? Evidence from six- and nine-year-olds’ production of passives. Journal of Child Language, 39 (5), 991–1016. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Messenger, K., Branigan, H. P., McLean, J. F., & Sorace, A.
(2012b) Is young children’s passive syntax semantically constrained? Evidence from syntactic priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 66 (4), 568–587. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Miller, C. A., & Deevy, P.
(2006) Structural priming in children with and without specific language impairment. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 20 (5), 387–399. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nitschke, S., Kidd, E., & Serratrice, L.
(2010) First language transfer and long-term structural priming in comprehension. Language and Cognitive Processes, 25 (1), 94–114. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nitschke, S., Serratrice, L., & Kidd, E.
(2014) The effect of linguistic nativeness on structural priming in comprehension. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 29 (5), 525–542. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Peter, M., Chang, F., Pine, J. M., Blything, R., & Rowland, C. F.
(2015) When and how do children develop knowledge of verb argument structure? Evidence from verb bias effects in a structural priming task. Journal of Memory and Language, 81 , 1–15. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pickering, M. J., & Branigan, H. P.
(1998) The representation of verbs: Evidence from syntactic priming in language production. Journal of Memory and Language, 39 , 633–651. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Qi, Z., Yuan, S., & Fisher, C.
(2011) Where does verb bias come from? Experience with particular verbs affects online sentence processing. In N. Danis, K. Mesh, & H. Sung (Eds.), BUCLD35: Proceedings of the 35th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (Vol. 2, pp. 500–512). Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Rowland, C. F., Chang, F., Ambridge, B., Pine, J. M., & Lieven, E. V. M.
(2012) The development of abstract syntax: Evidence from structural priming and the lexical boost. Cognition, 125 (1), 49–63. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Savage, C., Lieven, E., Theakston, A., & Tomasello, M.
(2003) Testing the abstractness of children’s linguistic representations: Lexical and structural priming of syntactic constructions in young children. Developmental Science, 6 (5), 557–567. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2006) Structural priming as implicit learning in language acquisition: The persistence of lexical and structural priming in 4-year-olds. Language Learning and Development, 2 (1), 27–49. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Scheepers, C., & Crocker, M. W.
(2004) Constituent order priming from reading to listening: A visual-world study. In M. Carreiras & C. Clifton Jr. (Eds.), The on-line study of sentence comprehension: Eyetracking, ERP and beyond (pp. 1–14). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Serratrice, L., Hesketh, A., & Ashworth, R.
(2015) The use of reported speech in children’s narratives: A priming study. First Language, 35 (1), 68–87. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Shimpi, P. M., Gámez, P. B., Huttenlocher, J., & Vasilyeva, M.
(2007) Syntactic priming in 3- and 4-year-old children: Evidence for abstract representations of transitive and dative forms. Developmental Psychology, 43 (6), 1334–1346. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Snedeker, J., & Trueswell, J. C.
(2004) The developing constraints on parsing decisions: The role of lexical-biases and referential scenes in child and adult sentence processing. Cognitive Psychology, 49 , 238–299. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Thothathiri, M., & Snedeker, J.
(2008a) Give and take: Syntactic priming during spoken language comprehension. Cognition, 108 (1), 51–68. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2008b) Syntactic priming during language comprehension in three- and four-year-old children. Journal of Memory and Language, 58 , 188–213. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tooley, K. M., & Bock, K.
(2014) On the parity of structural persistence in language production and comprehension. Cognition, 132 (2), 101–136. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tooley, K. M., & Traxler, M. J.
(2010) Syntactic priming effects in comprehension: A critical review. Language and Linguistics Compass, 4 (10), 925–937. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Traxler, M. J.
(2008) Lexically independent priming in online sentence comprehension. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15 (1), 149–155. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2014) Priming of early closure: evidence for the lexical boost during sentence comprehension. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 30 (4), 478–490. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Traxler, M. J., & Tooley, K. M.
(2008) Priming in sentence comprehension: Strategic or syntactic? Language and Cognitive Processes, 23 (5), 609–645. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
van Beijsterveldt, L. M., & van Hell, J. G.
(2009) Structural priming of adjective–noun structures in hearing and deaf children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 104 (2), 179–196. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Vasilyeva, M., Huttenlocher, J., & Waterfall, H.
(2006) Effects of language intervention on syntactic skill levels in preschoolers. Developmental Psychology, 42 (1), 164–174. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Vasilyeva, M., & Waterfall, H.
(2012) Beyond syntactic priming: Evidence for activation of alternative syntactic structures. Journal of Child Language, 39 (2), 258–283. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Zimmer, E. J.
(2017) Children’s comprehension of two types of syntactic ambiguity. First Language, 37 (1), 7–23. CrossrefGoogle Scholar