Article published in:
The architecture of writing systems
Edited by Kristian Berg, Franziska Buchmann and Nanna Fuhrhop
[Written Language & Literacy 17:2] 2014
► pp. 225252
References

References

Adelman, James S.
(2011) Letters in time and retinotopic space. Psychological Review 118: 570–582. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. Harald, Petar Milin, Dusica F. Durdevic, Peter Hendrix & Marco Marelli
(2011) An amorphous model for morphological processing in visual comprehension based on naive discriminative learning. Psychological Review 118: 438–481. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bar, Moshe, Karim S. Kassam, Avniel S. Ghuman, Jasmine Boshyan, Annette M. Schmidt, Anders M. Dale, Matti S. Hämäläinen, Ksenija Marinkovic, Daniel L. Schacter, Bruce R. Rosen & Eric Halgren
(2006) Top-down facilitation of visual recognition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 103: 449–454. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Biederman, Irving
(1987) Recognition-by-cComponents: A theory of human image understanding. Psychological Review 94: 115–147. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Binder, Jeffrey R., David Medler, Chris Westbury, Einat Liebenthal & Lori Buchanan
(2006) Tuning of the human left fusiform gyrus to sublexical orthographic structure. Neuroimage 33: 739–748. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bouma, Herman
(1971) Visual recognition of isolated lower-case letters. Vision Research 11: 459–474. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bouma, Herman & Charles P. Legein
(1977) Foveal and parafoveal recognition of letters and words by dyslexics and by average readers. Neuropsychologia 15: 69–80. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Brincat, Scott L. & Charles E. Connor
(2004) Underlying principles of visual shape selectivity in posterior inferotemporal cortex. Nature Neuroscience 7: 880–886. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cattell, James M.
(1886) The time it takes to see and name objects. Mind 11: 53–65.Google Scholar
Chanceaux, Myriam & Jonathan Grainger
(2012) Serial position effects in the identification of letters, digits, symbols, and shapes in peripheral vision. Acta Psychologica 141: 149–158. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chanceaux, Myriam, Françoise Vitu, Luisa Bendahman, Simon Thorpe & Jonathan Grainger
(2012) Word processing speed in peripheral vision measured with a saccadic choice task. Vision Research 56: 10–19. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chauncey, Krysta, Phillip J. Holcomb & Jonathan Grainger
(2008) Effects of stimulus font and size on masked repetition priming: An ERP investigation. Language and Cognitive Processes 23: 183–200. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chetail, Fabienne & Alain Content
(2012) The internal structure of chaos: Letter category determines visual word perceptual units. Journal of Memory and Language 67: 371–388. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, Laurent, Stanislas Dehaene, Lionel Naccache, Stéphane Lehéricy, Ghislaine Dehaene-Lambertz, Marie-Anne Hénaff & François Michel
(2000) The visual word form area: Spatial and temporal characterization of an initial stage of reading in normal subjects and posterior split-brain patients. Brain 123: 291–307. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Coltheart, Max, Kathy Rastle, Conrad Perry, Richard Langdon & Johannes C. Ziegler
(2001) DRC: A dual-route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review 108: 204–256. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dandurand, Frédéric, Jonathan Grainger, Jon-Andoni Duñabeitia & Jean-Pierre Granier
(2011) On coding non-contiguous letter combinations. Frontiers in Cognitive Science 2: 136.Google Scholar
Dandurand, Frédéric, Thomas Hannagan & Jonathan Grainger
(2013) Computational models of location-invariant orthographic processing. Connection Science 25: 1–26. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dehaene, Stanislas & Laurent Cohen
(2011) The unique role of the visual word form area in reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15: 254–262. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dehaene, Stanislas, Laurent Cohen, Mario Sigman & Fabien Vinckier
(2005) The neural code for written words: A proposal. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9: 335–341. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dehaene, Stanislas, Felipe Pegado, Lucia W. Braga, Paulo Ventura, Gilberto Nunes Filho, Antoinette Jobert, Ghislaine Dehaene-Lambertz, Régine Kolinsky, José Morais & Laurent Cohen
(2010) How learning to read changes the cortical networks for vision and language. Science 330: 1359–1364. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Diependaele, Kevin, Johannes C. Ziegler & Jonathan Grainger
(2010) Fast phonology and the bi-modal interactive activation model. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 22: 764–778. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Duñabeitia, Jon Andoni, Maria Dimitropoulou, Jonathan Grainger, Juan A. Hernández & Manuel Carreiras
(2012) Differential sensitivity of letters, numbers and symbols to character transpositions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 24: 1610–1624. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ehri, Linnea C.
(1992) Reconceptualizing the development of sight word reading and its relationship to recoding. In Philip B. Gough, Linnea E. Ehri & Rebecca Treiman (eds.), Reading acquisition, 105–143. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Fukushima, Kunihiko
(1980) Neocognitron: A self-organizing neural network for a mechanism of pattern recognition unaffected by shift in position. Biological Cybernetics 36: 193–202. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gil-López, Cristina, Manuel Perea, Carmen Moret-Tatay & Manuel Carreiras
(2011) Can masked priming effects be obtained with handwritten words? Attention, Perception & Psychophysics 73: 1643–1649. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Grainger, Jonathan
(2008) Cracking the orthographic code: An introduction. Language and Cognitive Processes 23: 1–35. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Grainger, Jonathan, Kevin Diependaele, Elsa Spinelli, Ludovic Ferrand & Fernand Farioli
(2003) Masked repetition and phonological priming within and across modalities. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition 29: 1256–1269. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Grainger, Jonathan & Stéphane Dufau
(2012) The front-end of visual word recognition. In James S. Adelman (ed.), Visual word recognition vol. 1: Models and methods, orthography and phonology. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Grainger, Jonathan, Stéphane Dufau, Marie Montant, Johannes C. Ziegler & Joel Fagot
(2012a) Orthographic processing in baboons (Papio papio). Science 336: 245–248. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Grainger, Jonathan & Ludovic Ferrand
(1994) Phonology and orthography in visual word recognition: Effects of masked homophone primes. Journal of Memory and Language 33: 218–233. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Grainger, Jonathan & Phillip J. Holcomb
(2009) Watching the word go by: On the time-course of component processes in visual word recognition. Language and Linguistics Compass 3: 128–156. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Grainger, Jonathan, Bernard Lété, Daisy Bertrand, Stéphane Dufau & Johannes C. Ziegler
(2012b) Evidence for multiple routes in learning to read. Cognition 123: 280–292. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Grainger, Jonathan, Arnaud Rey & Stéphane Dufau
(2008) Letter perception: From pixels to pandemonium! Trends in Cognitive Sciences 12: 381–387. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Grainger, Jonathan, Ilse Tydgat & Joanna Isselé
(2010) Crowding affects letters and symbols differently. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 36: 673–688. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Grainger, Jonathan & Walter van Heuven
(2003) Modeling letter position coding in printed word perception. In Patrick Bonin (ed.), The mental lexicon, 1–24. New York: Nova Science Publishers.Google Scholar
Grainger, Jonathan & Johannes C. Ziegler
(2008) Cross-code consistency effects in visual word recognition. In Elena L. Grigorenko & Adam J. Naples (eds.), Single-word reading: Biological and behavioral perspectives, 129–157. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
(2011) A dual-route approach to orthographic processing. Frontiers in Psychology 2: 54. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hannagan, Thomas & Jonathan Grainger
(2012) Protein analysis meets visual word recognition: A case for string kernels in the brain. Cognitive Science 36: 575–606. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2013) Learning diagnostic features: The delta rule does Bubbles. Journal of Vision 13.8: 17, 1–11. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hannagan, Thomas, Maria Ktori, Myriam Chanceaux & Jonathan Grainger
(2012) Deciphering CAPTCHAs: What a Turing test reveals about human cognition. PLoS One 7.3: e32121. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hannagan, Thomas, Johannes C. Ziegler, Stéphane Dufau, Joel Fagot & Jonathan Grainger
(2014) Deep learning of orthographic representations in baboons. PLoS One 9.1: e84843. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Haussler, David
(1999) Convolution kernels on discrete structure. Technical report UCSC-CRL-99-10, UC Santa Cruz.Google Scholar
Huey, Edmund B.
(1908) The psychology and pedagogy of reading. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Jäkel, Frank, Bernhard Schölkopf & Felix A. Wichmann
(2009) Does cognitive science need kernels? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13: 381–388. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kiyonaga, Kristi, Jonathan Grainger, Katherine J. Midgley & Phillip J. Holcomb
(2007) Masked cross-modal repetition priming: An event-related potential investigation. Language and Cognitive Processes 22: 337–376. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kobatake, Eucaly & Keiji Tanaka
(1994) Neuronal selectivities to complex object features in the ventral pathway of the macaque cerebral cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology 71: 856–857.Google Scholar
LeCun, Yann, Léon Bottou, Yoshua Bengio & Patrick Haffner
(1998) Gradient-based learning applied to document recognition. Proceedings of the IEEE 86: 2278–2324. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Leslie, Christina & Rui Kuang
(2004) Fast string kernels using inexact matching for protein sequences. Journal of Machine Learning Research 5: 1435–1455.Google Scholar
Lété, Bernard & Joël Pynte
(2003) Word-shape and word-lexical-frequency effects in lexical decision and naming tasks. Visual Cognition 10: 913–948. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Levi, Dennis M.
(2008) Crowding – an essential bottleneck for object recognition: A mini-review. Vision Research 48: 635–654. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lodhi, Huma, Craig Saunders, John Shawe-Taylor, Nello Cristianini & Chris Watkins
(2002) Text classification using string kernels. Journal of Machine Learning Research 2: 419–444.Google Scholar
Martelli, Marialuisa, Gloria Di Filippo, Donatella Spinelli & Pierluigi Zoccolotti
(2009) Crowding, reading, and developmental dyslexia. Journal of Vision 9: 1–18. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mason, Mildred
(1982) Recognition time for letters and nonletters: Effects of serial position, array size, and processing order. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 8: 724–738. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Massol, Stéphanie, Jon-Andoni Duñabeitia, Manuel Carreiras & Jonathan Grainger
(2013) Evidence for letter-specific position coding mechanisms. PLoS One 8.7: e68460. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Maurer, Urs, Silvia Brem, Kerstin Bucher, Felicitas Kranz, Rosmarie Benz, Hans-Christoph Steinhausen & Daniel Brandeis
(2006) Coarse neural tuning for print peaks when children learn to read. NeuroImage 33: 749–758. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, James L. & David E. Rumelhart
(1981) An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception. Part1: An account of basic findings. Psychological Review 88: 375–407. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mozer, Michael
(1987) Early parallel processing in reading: A connectionist approach. In Max Coltheart (ed.), Attention and perfomance XII: The psychology of reading, 83–104. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Paap, Kenneth R., Sandra L. Newsome & Ronald W. Noel
(1984) Word shape’s in poor shape for the race to the lexicon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 10: 413–428. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pelli, Denis G. & Katherine A. Tillman
(2008) The uncrowded window for object recognition. Nature Neuroscience 11: 1129–1135. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Perea, Manolo, Victoria Panadero, Carmen Moret-Tatay & Pablo Gómez
(2012) The effects of inter-letter spacing in visual-word recognition: Evidence with young normal readers and developmental dyslexics. Learning and Instruction 22: 420–430. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Perea, Manuel & Eva Rosa
(2002) Does ‘whole-word shape’ play a role in visual word recognition? Perception & Psychophysics 64: 785–794. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Perre, Laetitia & Johannes C. Ziegler
(2008) On-line activation of orthography in spoken word recognition. Brain Research 1188: 132–138. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Perry, Conrad, Johannes C. Ziegler & Marco Zorzi
(2007) Nested incremental modeling in the development of computational theories: the CDP+ model of reading aloud. Psychological Review 114: 273–315. Crossref
Plaut, David C. & Marlene Behrmann
(2011) Complementary neural representations for faces and words: A computational exploration. Cognitive Neuropsychology 28: 251–275. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Plaut, David C., James L. McClelland, Mark S. Seidenberg & Karalyn Patterson
(1996) Understanding normal and impaired word reading: Computational principles in quasi-regular domains. Psychological Review 103: 56–115. CrossrefX.103.1.56Google Scholar
Primus, Beatrice
(2004) A featural analysis of the Modern Roman Alphabet. Written Language and Literacy 7.2: 235–274. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ramscar, Michael & Daniel Yarlett
(2007) Linguistic self-correction in the absence of feedback: A new approach to the logical problem of language acquisition. Cognitive Science 31: 927–960. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rescorla, Robert A. & Allan R. Wagner
(1972) A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In Abraham H. Black & William Frederick Prokasy (eds.), Classical conditioning II, 64–99. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Rastle, Kathy & Marc Brysbaert
(2006) Masked phonological priming effects in English: Are they real? Do they matter? Cognitive Psychology 53: 97–145. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rumelhart, David E, Geoffrey E. Hinton & Ronald J. Williams
(1986) Learning representations by back-propagating errors. Nature 323: 533–536. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Seidenberg, Mark S. & James L. McClelland
(1989) A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. Psychological Review 96: 523–568. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Segui, Juan & Jonathan Grainger
(1990) Priming word recognition with orthographic neighbors: Effects of relative prime-target frequency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 16: 65–76. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Share, David L.
(1995) Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition 55: 151–218. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tsunoda, Kazushige, Yukako Yamane, Makoto Nishizaki & Manabu Tanifuji
(2001) Complex objects are represented in macaque inferotemporal cortex by the combination of feature columns. Nature Neuroscience 4: 832–838. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tydgat, Ilse & Jonathan Grainger
(2009) Serial position effects in the identification of letters, digits, and symbols. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 35: 480–498. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Vinckier, Fabien, Stanislas Dehaene, Mariano Sigman & Laurent Cohen
(2007) Hierarchical coding of letter strings in the ventral stream: Dissecting the inner organization of the visual wordform system. Neuron 55: 143–156. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Vinckier, Fabien, Emilie Qiao, Christophe Pallier, Stanislas Dehaene & Laurent Cohen
(2011) The impact of letter spacing on reading: A test of the bigram coding hypothesis. Journal of Vision 11: 1–21. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Walker, Peter
(1987) Word shape as a cue to the identity of a word: An analysis of the Kucera and Francis (1967) word list. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 39A: 675–700. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Whitney, Carol
(2001) How the brain encodes the order of letters in a printed word: The SERIOL model and selective literature review. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 8: 221–243. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Whitney, David & Dennis M. Levi
(2011) Visual crowding: A fundamental limit on conscious perception and object recognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15: 160–168. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Widrow, Bernard & Marcian E. Hoff
(1960) Adaptive switching circuits. In 1960 IRE WECON Convention Record Part 4, 96–104. New York, NY: IRE.Google Scholar
Ziegler, Johannes C. & Ludovic Ferrand
(1998) Orthography shapes the perception of speech: The consistency effect in auditory word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 5: 683–689. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ziegler, Johannes C., Thomas Hannagan, Stéphane Dufau, Marie Montant, Joël Fagot & Jonathan Grainger
(2013) Transposed letter effects reveal orthographic processing in baboons. Psychological Science 20.10: 1–3.Google Scholar
Ziegler, Johannes C., Conrad Perry & Marco Zorzi
(2014) Modelling reading development through phonological decoding and self-teaching: Implications for dyslexia. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 369: no. 1634.Google Scholar
Zorzi, Marco, Chiara Barbiero, Andrea Facoetti, Isabella Longciari, Marco Carrozzi, Marcella Montico, Laura Bravar, Florence George, Catherine Pech-Georgel & Johannes C. Ziegler
(2012) Extra-large letter spacing improves reading in dyslexia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 109.28: 11455–11459. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 18 other publications

Dotan, Dror & Naama Friedmann
2019. Separate mechanisms for number reading and word reading: Evidence from selective impairments. Cortex 114  pp. 176 ff. Crossref logo
Eddy, Marianna D., Jonathan Grainger, Phillip J. Holcomb & John D. E. Gabrieli
2016. Orthographic and phonological processing in developing readers revealed by ERPs. Psychophysiology 53:12  pp. 1776 ff. Crossref logo
Fernández‐López, María, Ana Marcet & Manuel Perea
2021. Does orthographic processing emerge rapidly after learning a new script?. British Journal of Psychology 112:1  pp. 52 ff. Crossref logo
Grainger, Jonathan
2018. Orthographic processing: A ‘mid-level’ vision of reading: The 44th Sir Frederic Bartlett Lecture. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 71:2  pp. 335 ff. Crossref logo
Grainger, Jonathan & Elisabeth Beyersmann
2017.  In [Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 67],  pp. 285 ff. Crossref logo
Grainger, Jonathan, Stéphane Dufau & Johannes C. Ziegler
2016. A Vision of Reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 20:3  pp. 171 ff. Crossref logo
Kerr, Emilia, Jonathan Mirault & Jonathan Grainger
2021. On non-adjacent letter repetition and orthographic processing: Lexical decisions to nonwords created by repeating or inserting letters in words. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 28:2  pp. 596 ff. Crossref logo
Ktori, Maria, Daisy Bertrand & Jonathan Grainger
2019. What’s special about orthographic processing? Further evidence from transposition effects in same-different matching. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 72:7  pp. 1780 ff. Crossref logo
Lafontaine, Hélène & Régine Kolinsky
2019. Reading by extracting invariant line junctions in typical and atypical young readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 183  pp. 75 ff. Crossref logo
Leibnitz, Laurie, Jonathan Grainger, Mathilde Muneaux & Stéphanie Ducrot
2017. Processus visuo-attentionnels et lecture : une synthèse. L’Année psychologique 116:04  pp. 597 ff. Crossref logo
Linke, Maja, Franziska Bröker, Michael Ramscar, Harald Baayen & Thomas Wennekers
2017. Are baboons learning "orthographic" representations? Probably not. PLOS ONE 12:8  pp. e0183876 ff. Crossref logo
Rakhlin, Natalia V., Catalina Mourgues, Cláudia Cardoso-Martins, Alexander N. Kornev & Elena L. Grigorenko
2019. Orthographic processing is a key predictor of reading fluency in good and poor readers in a transparent orthography. Contemporary Educational Psychology 56  pp. 250 ff. Crossref logo
Schubert, Teresa, Nicholas Badcock & Saskia Kohnen
2017. Development of children’s identity and position processing for letter, digit, and symbol strings: A cross-sectional study of the primary school years. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 162  pp. 163 ff. Crossref logo
Schubert, Teresa, Sachiko Kinoshita & Dennis Norris
2017. What Causes the Greater Perceived Similarity of Consonant-Transposed Nonwords?. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology  pp. 17470218.2016.1 ff. Crossref logo
Schubert, Teresa M.
2017. Why are digits easier to identify than letters?. Neuropsychologia 95  pp. 136 ff. Crossref logo
Tóth, Dénes & Valéria Csépe
2017. Adaptive specialization in position encoding while learning to read. Developmental Science 20:3  pp. e12426 ff. Crossref logo
Ventura, Paulo, Tânia Fernandes, Alexandre Pereira, José C. Guerreiro, António Farinha-Fernandes, João Delgado, Miguel F. Ferreira, Bruno Faustino, Isabel Raposo & Alan C.-N. Wong
2020. Holistic word processing is correlated with efficiency in visual word recognition. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 82:5  pp. 2739 ff. Crossref logo
Viterbo, Goni, Tami Katzir & Liat Goldfarb
2020. Accelerating reading via local priming. Acta Psychologica 205  pp. 103056 ff. Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 11 november 2021. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.