Article published in:
Colour Studies: A broad spectrum
Edited by Wendy Anderson, Carole P. Biggam, Carole Hough and Christian Kay
[Not in series 191] 2014
► pp. 169180
References
Berlin, Brent, and Paul Kay
1969Basic Color Terms: Their Universality and Evolution. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Belpaeme, Tony, and Joris Bleys
2009 “The Impact of Statistical Distributions of Colours on Colour Category Acquisition.” Journal of Cognitive Science 10: 1–20.Google Scholar
Bornstein, Marc
1976 “Infant Recognition Memory for Hue.” Developmental Psychology 12: 185–191. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bornstein, Marc, William Kessen, and Sally Weiskopf
1976“Color Vision and Hue Categorisation in Young Human Infants.” Journal of Experimental Psychology, Human Perception and Performance 2: 115–129. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bornstein, Marc, and N.O. Korda
1984 “Discrimination and Matching within and between Hues Measured by Reaction Times: Some Implications for Categorical Perception and Levels of Information Processing.” Psychological Research 46: 207–222. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Brown, Angela, M., Lindsey Delwin, and Kevin M. Guckes
2011 “Color Names, Color Categories and Color–cued Visual Search: Sometimes, Color Perception is not Categorical.” Journal of Vision 11: 1–21.Google Scholar
Catherwood, Dianne, Boris Crassini, and Kate Freiberg
1987 “The Nature of Infant Memory for Hue.” British Journal of Developmental Psychology 5: 385–394. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
1990 “The Course of Infant Memory for Hue.” Australian Journal of Psychology 42: 277–285. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Clifford, Alexandra, Anna Franklin, Ian R.L. Davies, and Amanda Holmes
2009 “Electrophysiological Markers of Color Categories in the Infant Brain.” Brain and Cognition 71: 165–172. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Clifford, Alexandra, Anna Franklin, Amanda Holmes, and Ian R.L. Davies
2011 “Investigating the Underlying Mechanisms of Categorical Perception of Colour Using the Event-Related Potential Technique.” In New Directions in Colour Studies, ed. by Carole P. Biggam, Carole A. Hough, Christian J. Kay, and David R. Simmons, 237–249. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Clifford, Alexandra, Anna Franklin, Amanda Holmes, Gilda V. Drivonikou, Emre Özgen, and Ian R.L. Davies
2012 “Neural Correlates of Acquired Color Category Effects.” Brain and Cognition 80: 126–143. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Davidoff, Jules
2001 “Language and Perceptual Categorisation.” Trends in Cognitive Sciences 5: 382–387. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Davidoff, Jules, and Joel Fagot
2010 “Cross–species Assessment of the Linguistic Origins of Color Categories.” Comparative Cognition and Behavior Reviews 5: 100–116. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Davidoff, Jules, Julie Goldstein, and Debi Roberson
2009 “Nature versus Nature: The Simple Contrast.” Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 102: 246–250. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Davies, Ian R.L., and Anna Franklin
2002 “Categorical Similarity may Affect Colour Pop-out in Infants After-all.” British Journal of Developmental Psychology 20: 185–203. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fagot, Joël, Julie Goldstein, Jules Davidoff, and Alan Pickering
2006 “Cross-species Differences in Color Categorization.” Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 13: 275–280. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Franklin, Anna
2009 “Pre-linguistic Categorical Perception of Colour cannot be Explained by Colour Preference: Response to Roberson and Hanley.” Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13: 501–502. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Franklin, Anna, and Ian R.L. Davies
2004 “New Evidence for Infant Colour Categories.” British Journal of Developmental Psychology 22: 349–377. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Franklin, Anna, Gilda V. Drivonikou, Laura Bevis, Ian R.L. Davies, Paul Kay, and Terry Regier
2008 “Categorical Perception of Color is Lateralized to the Right Hemisphere in Infants, but to the Left Hemisphere in Adults.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 105: 3221–3225. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Franklin, Anna, Emily Gibbons, Katie Chittenden, James Alvarez, and Chloe Taylor
2012 “Infant Color Preference for Red is not Selectively Context Specific.” Emotion 12: 1155–1160. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Franklin, Anna, Michael Pilling, and Ian R.L. Davies
2005 “The Nature of Infant Colour Categorisation: Evidence from Eye-movements on a Target Detection Task.” Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 91: 227–248. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gerhardstein, Peter, Peggy Renner, and Carolyn Rovee-Collier
1999 “The Roles of Perceptual and Categorical Similarity in C 1 Color Pop-out in Infants.” British Journal of Developmental Psychology 7: 403–420. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gilbert, Aubrey L., Terry Regier, Paul Kay, and Richard B. Ivry
2006 “Whorf Hypothesis is Supported in the Right Visual Field but not the Left.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 103: 489–494. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Harnad, Stevan
1987 “Psychophysical and Cognitive Aspects of Categorical Perception: A Critical Overview.” In Categorical Perception: The Groundwork of Cognition, ed. by Stevan Harnad, 287–301. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hespos, Susan J., and Elizabeth S. Spelke
2004 “Precursors to Spatial Language.” Nature 430: 453–456. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jameson, Kimberly A.
2005 “Why GRUE? An Interpoint-distance Model Analysis of Composite Color Categories.” Cross-Cultural Research 39: 159–194. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kay, Paul, Brent Berlin, Luisa Maffi, William R Merrifield, and Richard Cook
2009. The World Colour Survey. Stanford: Center for the Study of Language and Information.Google Scholar
Kay, Paul, and Terry Regier
2003 “Resolving the Question of Color Naming Universals.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 100: 9085–9089. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Knoblauch, Kenneth, François Vital-Durand, and John L. Barbur
2001 “Variation of Chromatic Sensitivity across the Life Span.” Vision Research 41: 23–26. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kowalski, Kurt, and Herbert Zimiles
2006 “The Relation between Children’s Conceptual Functioning with Color and Color Term Acquisition.” Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 94: 301–321. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Martin-Malivel, Julie, Michael C. Mangini, Joël Fagot, and Irving Biederman
2006 “Do Humans and Baboons Use the Same Information when Categorizing Human and Baboon Faces?Psychological Science 17: 599–607. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ozturk, Ozge, Shakila Shayan, Ulf Liszkowski, and Asifa Majid
2013 “Language is not Necessary for Color Categories.” Developmental Science 16: 111–115. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pascalis, Olivier, and Michelle de Haan
2003 “Recognition Memory and Novelty Preference: What Model?” In Progress in Infancy Research 3, ed. by H. Hayne, and J. Fagen, 95–120. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Philipona, David L., and Kevin O’Regan
2006 “Color Naming, Unique Hues and Hue Cancellation Predicted from Singularities in Reflection Properties.” Visual Neuroscience 23: 331–339. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pitchford, Nicola J., and Kathy Mullen
2001 “Conceptualization of Perceptual Attributes: A Special Case for Color?Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 80: 289–314. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Quinn, Paul, Alissa Westerlund, and Charles A. Nelson
2006 “Neural Markers of Categorization in 6-month-old Infants.” Psychological Science 17: 59–66. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rakison, David H., and Yevdokiya Yermolayeva
2010 “Infant Categorization.” Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science 1: 894–905. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Regier, Terry, and Paul Kay
2009 “Language, Thought and Color: Whorf was Half Right.” Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13: 439–446. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Regier, Terry, Paul Kay, and Richard S. Cook
2005 “Focal Colors are Universal after all.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 102: 8386–8391. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Regier, Terry, Paul Kay, and Naveen Khetarpal
2007 “Color Naming Reflects Optimal Partitions of Color Space.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 104: 1436–1441. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Roberson, Debi, and Jules Davidoff
2000 “The Categorical Perception of Colors and Facial Expressions: The Effect of Verbal Interference.” Memory and Cognition 28: 977–986. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Roberson, Debi, and J. Richard Hanley
2009 “Only Half Right: Comment on Regier and Kay.” Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13: 500–501. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ruiz, M.J., and J.M. Hupé
2011 “Stimulus Saliency, not Colour Category Boundary, Accounts for ‘Whorfian’ Effects in Colour Search Tasks.” Perception 40: 196. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Werker, Janet F., and Richard C. Tees
1984 “Cross-language Speech Perception: Evidence for Perceptual Reorganization during the First Year of Life.” Infant Behavior and Development 7: 49–63. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Werner, John S., and B.R. Wooten
1985 “Unsettled Issues in Infant Color Vision.” Behavior and Development 8: 99–107. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Witzel, Christoph, and Karl R. Gegenfurtner
2011 “Is there a Lateralized Category Effect for Color?Journal of Vision 11: 1–25. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wright, Anthony, and William Cumming
1971 “Color-naming Functions for the Pigeon.” Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 15: 7–17. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Xiao, Youping, Christopher Kavanau, Lauren Bertin, and Ehud Kaplan
2011 “The Biological Basis of a Universal Constraint on Color Naming: Cone Contrasts and the Two-way Categorization of Colors.” PLoS ONE 6: e24994. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Yendrikhovskij, Sergej N.
2001 “Computing Color Categories from Statistics of Natural Images.” Journal of Imaging Science and Technology 45: 409–41.Google Scholar