The neural basis for primary and acquired language skills
Reading is a cultural invention that needs
to recruit cortical infrastructure that was not designed for it
(cultural recycling of cortical maps). In the case of reading both
visual cortex and networks for speech processing are recruited. Here
I discuss current views on the neurobiological underpinnings of
spoken language that deviate in a number of ways from the classical
Wernicke-Lichtheim-Geschwind model. More areas than Broca’s and
Wernicke’s region are involved in language. Moreover, a division
along the axis of language production and language comprehension
does not seem to be warranted. Instead, for central aspects of
language processing neural infrastructure is shared between
production and comprehension. Arguments are presented in favor of a
dynamic network view, in which the functionality of a region is
co-determined by the network of regions in which it is embedded at
particular moments in time. Finally, core regions of language
processing need to interact with other networks (e.g. the
attentional networks and the ToM network) to establish full
functionality of language and communication. The consequences of
this architecture for reading are discussed.
Article outline
- 1.Introduction
- 2.The neurobiology of language
- 3.Connectivity in the language network
- 4.Beyond the classical model
- 5.Reading as an extension of the language network
-
Acknowledgements
-
References
References (37)
References
Amunts, K., & Catani, M. (2015). Cytoarchitectonics, receptorarchitectonics, and network topology of language. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (5th ed.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Binder, J. R., & Desai, R. H. (2011). The neurobiology of semantic memory. Trends in cognitive sciences, 15(11), 527–536.
Blomert, L. (2011). The neural signature of orthographic-phonological binding in successful and failing reading development. Neuroimage, 57(3), 695–703.
Bozic, M., Tyler, L. K., Ives, D. T., Randall, B., & Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (2010). Bihemispheric foundations for human speech comprehension. PNAS, 107(40), 17439–17444.
Caramazza, A., & Zurif, E. B. (1976). Dissociation of algorithmic and heuristic processes in language comprehension: Evidence from aphasia. Brain and Language, 3(4), 572–582.
Duncan, J. (2010). How intelligence happens. New Haven and London: Yale University Press.
Fedorenko, E., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2014). Reworking the language network. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(3), 120–126.
Friederici, A. D. (2002). Towards a neural basis of auditory sentence processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 78–84.
Friederici, A. D. (2012). The cortical language circuit: From auditory perception to sentence comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 78–84.
Hagoort, P. (2003). Interplay between syntax and semantics during sentence comprehension: ERP effects of combining syntactic and semantic violations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(6), 883–899.
Hagoort, P. (2005). On Broca, brain, and binding: A new framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 416–423.
Hagoort, P. (2009). The fractionation of spoken language understanding by measuring electrical and magnetic brain signals. In B. C. J. Moore, L. K. Tyler & W. Marslen-Wilson (Eds.), The perception of speech (pp. 223–248). New York: Oxford University Press.
Hagoort, P. (2013). MUC (Memory, Unification, Control) and beyond. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(Article 416).
Hagoort, P. (2014). Nodes and networks in the neural architecture for language: Broca’s region and beyond. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 28C, 136–141.
Hagoort, P., & Indefrey, P. (2014). The Neurobiology of language beyond single words. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 37(1).
Hagoort, P., & Levelt, W. J. M. (2009). The speaking brain. Science, 326(5951), 372–373.
Hamzei, F., Rijntjes, M., Dettmers, C., Glauche, V., Weiller, C., & Buchel, C. (2003). The human action recognition system and its relationship to Broca’s area: An fMRI study. Neuroimage, 19, 637–644.
Hickok, G., & Poeppel, D. (2007). The cortical organization of speech processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8(5), 393–402.
Jackendoff, R. (2002). Foundations of language: Brain, meaning, grammar, evolution. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Johansen-Berg, H. (2013). Human connectomics - What will the future demand? NeuroImage, 80, 541–544.
Kunert, R., Willems, R. M., Casasanto, D., Patel, A. D., & Hagoort, P. (2015). Music and language syntax interact in Broca’s area: An fMRI study. PLoS ONE, 10(11), e0141069.
Lamme, V. A., & Roelfsema, P. R. (2000). The distinct modes of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing. Trends in Neurosciences, 23(11), 571–579.
Levelt, W. J. M. (2013). A history of psycholinguistics: The pre-Chomskyan era. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
McIntosh, A. R. (2008). Large-scale network dynamics in neurocognitive function. In A. Fuchs & V. K. Jirsa (Eds.), Coordination: Neural, behavioral and social dynamics (pp. 183–204). Springer Berlin Heidelberg.
Menenti, L., Gierhan, S. M. E., Segaert, K., & Hagoort, P. (2011). Shared language: Overlap and segregation of the neuronal infrastructure for speaking and listening revealed by functional MRI. Psychological Science, 22, 1173–1182.
Mesulam, M.-M. (1998). From sensation to cognition. Brain, 121, 1013–1052.
Nowak, M. (2011). Super cooperators: Beyond the survival of the fittest. Why cooperation, not competition is the key to life. Edinburgh-London: Canongate.
Patel, A. D. (2003). Language, music, syntax and the brain. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 674–681.
Petersson, K. M., & Hagoort, P. (2012). The neurobiology of syntax: Beyond string-sets. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 367, 1971–1883.
Price, C. J. (2010). The anatomy of language: A review of 100 fMRI studies published in 2009. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1191, 62–88.
Pulvermuller, F. (2013). How neurons make meaning: Brain mechanisms for embodied and abstract-symbolic semantics. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(9), 458–470.
Sahin, N. T., Pinker, S., Cash, S. S., Schomer, D., & Halgren, E. (2009). Sequential processing of lexical, grammatical, and phonological information within broca’s area. Science, 326(5951), 445–449.
Schuit, M. van der. (2011). Enhancing early language development in children with intellectual disabilities. Nijmegen: Radboud University Nijmegen.
Segaert, K., Menenti, L., Weber, K., Petersson, K. M., & Hagoort, P. (2012). Shared syntax in language production and language comprehension-an FMRI study. Cerebral Cortex, 22(7), 1662–1670.
Sporns, O. (2011). Networks of the brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Turken, A. U., & Dronkers, N. F. (2011). The neural architecture of the language comprehension network: Converging evidence from lesion and connectivity analyses. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 5, 1.
Xiang, H., Fonteijn, H. M., Norris, D. G., & Hagoort, P. (2010). Topographical functional connectivity pattern in the Perisylvian language networks. Cerebral Cortex, 20, 549–560.
Cited by (2)
Cited by two other publications
Mulder, Evelien, Marco van de Ven, Eliane Segers, Alexander Krepel, Elise H. de Bree, Peter F. de Jong & Ludo Verhoeven
2024.
Impact of word‐to‐text integration processes on reading comprehension development in English as a second language.
Journal of Research in Reading 47:1
► pp. 83 ff.
Verhoeven, Ludo & Charles Perfetti
2022.
Universals in Learning to Read Across Languages and Writing Systems.
Scientific Studies of Reading 26:2
► pp. 150 ff.
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 30 september 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.