Part of
Developmental Perspectives in Written Language and Literacy: In honor of Ludo Verhoeven
Edited by Eliane Segers and Paul van den Broek
[Not in series 206] 2017
► pp. 315331
References
Arciuli, J., & Simpson, I
(2012) Statistical learning is related to reading ability in children and adults. Cognitive Science, 36, 286–304. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brus, B., & Voeten, M
(1973) Eén-Minuut-Test, Versie A [One-Minute-Test, Version A]. Nijmegen: Berkhout.Google Scholar
Evans, J. L., Saffran, J. R., & Robe-Torres, K
(2009) Statistical learning in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52, 321–335. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Frost, R., Armstrong, B. C., Siegelman, N., & Christiansen, M. H
(2015) Domain generality versus modality specificity: the paradox of statistical learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19, 117–125. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gómez, R. L
(2002) Variability and detection of invariant structure. Psychological Science, 13, 431–436. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gómez, R. L., & Maye, J
(2005) The developmental trajectory of nonadjacent dependency learning. Infancy, 7, 183–206. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grunow, H., Spaulding, T. J., Gómez, R. L., & Plante, E
(2006) The effects of variation on learning word order rules by adults with and without language-based learning disabilities. Journal of Communication Disorders, 39, 158–170. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hsu, H. J., & Bishop, D. V
(2010) Grammatical difficulties in children with specific language impairment: Is learning deficient? Human Development, 53, 264–277. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2014) Sequence‐specific procedural learning deficits in children with specific language impairment. Developmental Science, 17, 352–365. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hsu, H. J., Tomblin, B. J., & Christiansen, M. H
(2014) Impaired statistical learning of non-adjacent dependencies in adolescents with specific language impairment. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1–10. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kelly, S. W., Griffiths, S., & Frith, U
(2002) Evidence for implicit sequence learning in dyslexia. Dyslexia, 8, 43–52. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kerkhoff, A., de Bree, E., de Klerk, M., & Wijnen, F. N. K
(2013) Non-adjacent dependency learning in infants at familial risk of dyslexia. Journal of Child Language, 40, 11–28. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kuijpers, C., van der Leij, A., Been, P., van Leeuwen, Y., ter Keurs, M., Schreuder, R., & Van den Bos, K. P
(2003) Leesproblemen in het voortgezet onderwijs en de volwassenheid. Pedagogische Studiën, 80, 272–287.Google Scholar
Lum, J. A., Conti-Ramsden, G., Morgan, A. T., & Ullman, M. T
(2014) Procedural learning deficits in specific language impairment (SLI): A meta-analysis of serial reaction time task performance. Cortex, 51, 1–10. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lum, J. A., Ullman, M. T., & Conti-Ramsden, G
(2013) Procedural learning is impaired in dyslexia, evidence from a meta-analysis of serial reaction time studies. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34, 3460–3476. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McArthur, G. M., Hogben, J. H., Edwards, V. T., Heath, S. M., & Mengler, E. D
(2000) On the ‘specifics’ of specific reading disability and specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 869–874. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Misyak, J. B., & Christiansen, M. H
(2012) Statistical learning and language: An individual differences study. Language Learning, 62, 302–331. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nicolson, R., & Fawcett, A
(2007) Procedural learning difficulties: Reuniting the developmental disorders? Trends in Neurosciences, 30, 135–141. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nissen, M. J., & Bullemer, P
(1987) Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 1–32. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pavlidou, E. V., Kelly, M. L., & Williams, J. M
(2010) Do children with developmental dyslexia have impairments in implicit learning? Dyslexia, 16, 143–161. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Plante, E., Gómez, R., & Gerken, L
(2002) Sensitivity to word order cues by normal and language/learning disabled adults. Journal of Communication Disorders, 35, 453–462. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ramus, F., Rosen, S., Dakin, S. C., Day, B. L., Castellote, J. M., White, S., & Frith, U
(2003) Theories of developmental dyslexia: Insights from a multiple case study of dyslexic adults. Brain, 126, 841–865. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rispens, J., & Been, P
(2007) Subject-verb agreement and phonological processing in developmental dyslexia and SLI: A closer look. International Journal of Communication Disorders, 42, 293–305. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Robertson, E., & Joanisse, M
(2010) Spoken sentence comprehension in children with dyslexia and language impairment: The roles of syntax and working memory. Applied Psycholinguistics, 31, 141–165. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Saffran, J. R., Newport, E. L., Aslin, R. N., Tunick, R. A., & Barrueco, S
(1997) Incidental language learning: Listening (and learning) out of the corner of your ear. Psychological science, 8, 101–105. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Saffran, J. R
(2002) Constraints on statistical language learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 47, 172–196. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Snowling, M. J
(2000) Dyslexia. Oxford, England: Blackwell Publishers Ltd.Google Scholar
Tomblin, J. B., Mainela-Arnold, E., & Zhang, X
(2007) Procedural learning in adolescents with and without specific language impairment. Journal of Child Language Learning and Development, 3(4), 269–293. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ullman, M
(2004) Contributions of memory circuits to language: The declarative/procedural model. Cognition, 92, 231–270. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ullman, M. T., & Pierpont, E. I
(2005) Specific language impairment is not specific to language: The procedural deficit hypothesis. Cortex, 41, 399–433. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Uterwijk, J
(2000) WAIS-III Nederlandstalige bewerking. Technische handleiding [WAIS-III Dutch version. Technical manual]. Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.Google Scholar
Van den Bos, K., lutje Spelberg, H., Scheepstra, A., & de Vries, J
(1994) De Klepel, Vorm A en B. Een test voor de leesvaardigheid van pseudowoorden [De Klepel, Form A and B. A test for the readability of pseudo words]. Nijmegen: Berkhout.Google Scholar
Veenker, T
(1998) FEP. Experimental script for UiL-OTS Labs. Ms.
Vellutino, F. R., Fletcher, J. M., Snowling, M. J., & Scanlon, D. M
(2004) Specific reading disability (dyslexia): What have we learned in the past four decades? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 2–40. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wilsenach, C., & Wijnen, F
(2004) Perceptual sensitivity to morphosyntactic agreement in language learners. Evidence from Dutch children at risk for developing dyslexia. Proceedings of 28th BU Conference on Language Development (pp. 645–656). Somerville (Mass): Cascadilla Press.
Wijnen, F. N. K
(2013) Acquisition of linguistic categories: Cross-domain convergences. In J. Bolhuis & M. Everaert (Eds.), Birdsong, speech and language: Converging mechanisms (pp. 157–178). Cambridge, MA: MIT press.Google Scholar