Part of
Atypical Language Development in Romance Languages
Edited by Eva Aguilar-Mediavilla, Lucía Buil-Legaz, Raúl López-Penadés, Victor A. Sanchez-Azanza and Daniel Adrover-Roig
[Not in series 223] 2019
► pp. 113126
References (23)
References
Ackerman, B. P. 1982. On comprehending idioms: Do children get the picture? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 33, 439–454. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
American Psychiatric Association. 2013. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM- 5th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Andrés-Roqueta, C., & Katsos, N. 2017. The contribution of grammar, vocabulary and Theory of Mind in pragmatic language competence in children with autistic spectrum disorders. Frontiers in Psychology 8, 996. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M. 2004. Diagnostic dilemmas in specific language impairment. In L. Verhoeven & J. van Balkom (Eds.), Classification of developmental language disorders (pp. 309–326). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M., & Adams, C. 1989. Conversational characteristics of children with semantic-pragmatic disorder, II: What features lead to a judgement of inappropriacy? British Journal of Disorders of Communication 24 (3): 241–263. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M., & Norbury, C. F. 2002. Exploring the borderlands of autistic disorder and specific language impairment: A study using standardised diagnostic instruments. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 43(7), 917–929. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M., & Rosenbloom, L. 1987. Childhood language disorders: Classification and overview. In W. Yule & M. Rutter (Eds.), Language development and disorders. London: MacKeith Press.Google Scholar
Conti-Ramsden, G., Botting, N., & Knox, E. 2001. Follow-up of children attending infant language units: outcomes at 11 years of age. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders 36(2), 207–219. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gernsbacher, M. A., & Robertson, R. R. W. 1999. The role of suppression in figurative language comprehension. Journal of Pragmatics 31, 1616–1630. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Glucksberg, S. 2006. Beyond literal meanings: The psychology of allusion. Psychological Science 2(3), 146–152. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kerbel, D., & Grunwell, P. 1998a. A study of idiom comprehension in children with semantic-pragmatic difficulties, Part I: Task effects on the assessment of idiom comprehension in children. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders 33, 1–22. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
1998b. A study of idiom comprehension in children with semantic-pragmatic difficulties, Part II: Between-groups results and discussion. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders 33, 23–44. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Levorato, M. C., Nesi, B., & Cacciari, C. 2004. Reading comprehension and understanding idiomatic expressions: A developmental study. Brain and Language 9, 303–314. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mendoza, E., Carballo, G., Muñoz, J., & Fresneda, M. D. 2005. Test de comprensión de estructuras gramaticales ( CEG ). Madrid: TEA Ediciones.Google Scholar
Nippold, M. A., & Duthie, J. K. 2003. Mental imagery and idiom comprehension: A comparison of school-age children and adults. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 46, 788–799. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Norbury, C. F. 2004. Factors supporting idiom comprehension in children with communication disorders. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 47, 1179–1193. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pouscoulous, N. 2014. The elevator’s buttocks. Metaphorical abilities in children. In D. Matthews (Ed.), Pragmatic development in first language acquisition (pp. 239–259). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Raven, J., Raven, J. C., & Court, J. H. 1998. Coloured progressive matrices. Oxford: Oxford Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Rinaldi, W. 2000. Pragmatic comprehension in secondary school-aged students with specific developmental language disorder. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders 35, 1–29. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Saborit, C., & Julián, J. P. 2005. L’avaluació del llenguatge infantil. ELI. Universitat Jaume I de Castelló (UJI). Col·lecció: Educació.Google Scholar
Titone, D. A., & Connine, C. M. 1994. Descriptive norms for 171 idiomatic expressions: Familiarity, compositionality, predictability, and literality. Metaphor and Symbolic Activity 9, 247–270. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vance, M., & Wells, B. 1994. The wrong end of the stick: Language-impaired children’s understanding of nonliteral language. Child Language Teaching and Therapy 10, 23–46. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Winner, E., & Leekman, S. 1991. Distinguishing irony from deception: Understanding the speaker’s second-order intention. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 9, 257–270. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by (1)

Cited by one other publication

Andrés-Roqueta, Clara, Raquel Flores-Buils & Alfonso Igualada
2024. Validation of PleaseApp: a digital tool for the assessment of receptive pragmatic abilities in children with neurodevelopmental disorders. Frontiers in Psychiatry 15 DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 5 july 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.