References
Anderson, A. H., Bader, M., Bard, E. G., Boyle, E., Doherty, G., Garrod, S., & Sotillo, C. (1991). The HCRC map task corpus. Language and Speech, 34(4), 351–366.
Baayen, R. H. (2008). Analyzing Linguistic Data. A Practical Introduction to Statistics Using R. Cambridge University Press.
Baayen, R. H., van Rij, J., de Cat, C. and Wood, S. N. (to appear) Autocorrelated errors in experimental data in the language sciences: Some solutions offered by Generalized Additive Mixed Models. In Speelman, D., Heylen, K. and Geeraerts, D. (eds), Mixed Effects Regression Models in Linguistics. Berlin, Springer. Retrieved from [URL].
Bates, D., Kliegl, R., Vasishth, S. and Baayen, R. H. (submitted) Parsimonious mixed models.
Bates, E., & Liu, H. (1996). Cued shadowing. Language and Cognitive Processes, 11(6), 577–582.
Bentum, M., Ernestus, M., ten Bosch, L. & van den Bosch, A. (submitted) How do speech registers differ in the predictability of words?
Benzeghiba, M., De Mori, R., Deroo, O., Dupont, S., Erbes, T., Jouvet, D., Fissore, L., Laface, P., Mertins, A., Ris, S., Rose, R., Tyagi, V., & Wellekens, C. (2007). Automatic speech recognition and speech variability: A review. Speech Communication, 49(10), 763–786.
Bernhard, D., & Tucker, B. (2015). The effects of duration on human processing of reduced speech. Canadian Acoustics, 43(3).
Biber, D. (1988). Variation across speech and writing. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Biber, D., Conrad, S., & Reppen, R. (1998). Corpus Linguistics: Investigating Language Structure and Use. Cambridge University Press.
Brand, Sophie & Ernestus, Mirjam. (submitted) How do native listeners and learners of French comprehend French word pronunciation variants?
Brenner, D. (2013). The acoustics of Mandarin tones in careful and conversational speech. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 134(5), 4246.
Brenner, D. S. (2015). The phonetics of Mandarin tones in conversation. Retrieved from [URL]
Brouwer, S., Mitterer, H., & Huettig, F. (2012). Speech reductions change the dynamics of competition during spoken word recognition. Language and Cognitive Processes, 27(4), 539–571.
Bürki, A., Ernestus, M., Gendrot, C., Fougeron, C., & Frauenfelder, U. H. (2011). What affects the presence versus absence of schwa and its duration: A corpus analysis of French connected speech. The journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 130(6), 3980–3991.
Bürki, A., Ernestus, M. & Frauenfelder, U. H. (2010). Is there only one “fenêtre” in the production lexicon? On-line evidence on the nature of phonological representations of pronunciation variants for French schwa words. Journal of Memory and Language 62, 421–437.
Çetin, Ö., & Shriberg, E. (2006). Speaker overlaps and ASR errors in meetings: Effects before, during, and after the overlap. In 2006 IEEE International Conference on Acoustics Speech and Signal Processing Proceedings (Vol. 1).
Chen, T.-Y., & Tucker, B. V. (2013). Sonorant Onset Pitch as a Perceptual Cue of Lexical Tones in Mandarin. Phonetica, 70(3), 207–239.
Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the Theory of Syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Connine, C. M., & Titone, D. (1996). Phoneme monitoring. Language and Cognitive Processes, 11(6), 635–646.
De Chat, C. (2007). French Dislocation. Interpretation, syntax, acquisition. Oxford Studies in Theoretical Linguistics, 17. Oxford University Press, Oxford, (288pp).
Dilts, P. C. (2013). Modelling phonetic reduction in a corpus of spoken English using Random Forests and Mixed-Effects Regression (Thesis). Retrieved from [URL]
Drijvers, L., & Özyürek, A. (in press) Visual context enhanced: The joint contribution of iconic gestures and visible speech to degraded speech comprehension. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research.
Engen, K. J. V., Baese-Berk, M., Baker, R. E., Choi, A., Kim, M., & Bradlow, A. R. (2010). The Wildcat Corpus of Native-and Foreign-accented English: Communicative efficiency across conversational dyads with varying language alignment profiles. Language and Speech, 53(4), 510–540.
Ernestus, M. (2000). Voice assimilation and segment reduction in casual Dutch: A corpus-based study of the phonology-phonetic interface. Holland Institute of Generative Linguistics, Utrecht.
Ernestus, M. (2012). Message related variation: Segmental within speaker variation. In: A. C. Cohn, C. Fougeron, & M. Huffman (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Laboratory Phonology, 92–102. OUP, Oxford.
Ernestus, M. & R. H. Baayen. (2011). Corpora and exemplars in phonology. In: J. Goldsmith, J. Riggle, & A. Yu (eds.), The Handbook of Phonological Theory (2nd ed.), pages 374–400. Wiley-Blackwell, Chichester, West Sussex.
Ernestus, M., Baayen, R. H., & Schreuder, R. (2002). The recognition of reduced word forms. Brain and Language, 81, 162–173.
Ernestus, M., Hanique, I., & Verboom, E. (2015). The effect of speech situation on the occurrence of reduced word pronunciation variants. Journal of Phonetics, 48, 60–75.
Ernestus, M., Lahey, M., Verhees, F., & Baayen, R. H. (2006). Lexical frequency and voice assimilation. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 120, 1040–1051.
Fowler, C. A., & Turvey, M. T. (1981). Immediate compensation in bite-block speech. Phonetica, 37(5–6), 306–326.
Fu, Q.; Zeng, F. (2000). Identification of temporal envelop cues in Chinese tone recognition. Asia Pacific Journal of Speech Language and Hearing, 5: 45–57.
Gahl, S., Yao, Y., & Johnson, K. (2012). Why reduce? Phonological neighborhood density and phonetic reduction in spontaneous speech. Journal of Memory and Language, 66(4), 789–806.
Galliano, S., Georois, E., Mostefa, D., Choukri, K., Bonastre, J.-F., and Gravier, J. (2005). ESTER phase II evaluation campaign for the rich transcription of French broadcast news. Proc. Interspeech 2005, 2453–2456.
Gaskell, Gareth and William Marslen-Wilson. (1998). Mechanisms of phonological inference in speech perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 24: 380–396.
Gaygen, D. E., & Luce, P. A. (1998). Effects of modality on subjective frequency estimates and processing of spoken and printed words. Perception & psychophysics, 60(3), 465–483.
Gick, B. (2002). The use of ultrasound for linguistic phonetic fieldwork. Journal of the International Phonetic Association, 32(02), 113–121.
Godfrey, J. J., Holliman, E. C., & McDaniel, J. (1992). SWITCHBOARD: Telephone speech corpus for research and development. In 1992 IEEE International Conference on Acoustics, Speech, and Signal Processing, 1992. ICASSP-92 (Vol. 1, pp. 517–520).
Greenberg, S. (1999). Speaking in shorthand – A syllable-centric perspective for understanding pronunciation variation. Speech Communication, 29, 159–176.
Goldinger, S. D., & Papesh, M. H. (2012). Pupil dilation reflects the creation and retrieval of memories. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(2), 90–95.
Hastie, T. J. & Tibshirani, R. J. (2002). Generalized additive models. Vol. 43. CRC Press, 1990.
Heylighen, F., & Dewaele, J.-M. (2002). Variation in the contextuality of language: An empirical measure. Foundations of Science, 7(3), 293–340.
Hockett, Charles F. (1955). A manual of phonology. Baltimore: Waverly Press.
Hymes, D. (1992). The concept of communicative competence revisited. Thirty years of linguistic evolution. Studies in honour of René Dirven on the occasion of his sixtieth birthday, 31–57.
Kemps, R., Ernestus, M., Schreuder, R., & Baayen, R. H. (2004). Processing reduced word forms: The suffix restoration effect. Brain and Language, 19, 117–127.
Klingner, J., Tversky, B., & Hanrahan, P. (2011). Effects of visual and verbal presentation on cognitive load in vigilance, memory, and arithmetic tasks. Psychophysiology, 48(3), 323–332.
Koch, X., & Janse, E. (2016). Speech rate effects on the processing of conversational speech across the adult life span. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 139(4), 1618–1636.
Kruschke, J. K. (2010). What to believe: Bayesian methods for data analysis. Trends in cognitive sciences, 14(7), 293–300.
Kruschke, J. K. (2014). Doing Bayesian data analysis: A tutorial with R, JAGS, and Stan. Academic Press.
Kryuchkova, T., Tucker, B. V., Wurm, L. H., & Baayen, R. H. (2012). Danger and usefulness are detected early in auditory lexical processing: Evidence from electroencephalography. Brain and Language, 122(2), 81–91.
Labov, W. (1972). Sociolinguistic Patterns. University of Pennsylvania Press.
Lahiri, A., & Reetz, H. (2002). ‘Underspecified recognition’, in Carlos Gussenhoven, Natasha Warner, and Toni Rietveld (eds.), Phonology & Phonetics: Laboratory Phonology VII. Berlin, Mouton, pp. 637–676.
Levelt, W. J. M., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 1–38.
Lindblom, B. (1963). Spectrographic study of vowel reduction. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 35(11), 1773–1781.
Liu, S.; Samuel, A. G. (2004). Perception of Mandarin lexical tones when F0 information is neutralized. Language & Speech, 47: 109–138.
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES Project: Tools for analyzing talk. Third Edition. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
McLennan, C. T., Luce, P. A., & Charles-Luce, J. (2003). Representation of lexical form. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29(4), 539–553.
McQueen, J. (1996). Word spotting. Language and Cognitive Processes, 11(6), 695–699.
Mehta, G., & Cutler, A. (1988). Detection of target phonemes in spontaneous and read speech. Language and Speech, 31 (Pt 2), 135–156.
Mirman, D., Dixon, J. A., & Magnuson, J. S. (2008). Statistical and computational models of the visual world paradigm: Growth curves and individual differences. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 475–494.
Mulder, K., ten Bosch, L., & Boves, L. (submitted) Comparing different methods for analyzing ERP signals.
Munson, B., & Solomon, N. P. (2004). The Effect of Phonological Neighborhood Density on Vowel Articulation. J Speech Lang Hear Res, 47(5), 1048–1058.
Oleson, J. J., Cavanaugh, J. E., McMurray, B., & Brown, G. (2015). Detecting time-specific differences between temporal nonlinear curves: Analyzing data from the visual world paradigm. Statistical Methods in Medical Research, 0962280215607411.
Oostdijk, N. (2000). The Spoken Dutch Corpus Project. The ELRA Newsletter, 5, 4–8.
Pitt, M. A., Dilley, L., Johnson, K., Kiesling, S., Raymond, W., Hume, E., & Fosler-Lussier, E. (2007). Buckeye Corpus of Conversational Speech (2nd release) [[URL]] Columbus, OH: Department of Psychology. Ohio State University (Distributor).
Pluymaekers, M., Ernestus, M., & Baayen, R. (2006). Articulatory planning is continuous and sensitive to informational redundancy. Phonetica, 62(2–4), 146–159.
Podlubny, R., Geeraert, K., Tucker, B. V. (2015). It’s all about, like, acoustics. Proceedings of the 18th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences. Glasgow, UK: the University of Glasgow. Paper number 0477.
Podlubny, R., Tucker, B. V., & Nearey, T. (2011). ‘Sorry, what was that?’: The roles of pitch, duration, and amplitude in the perception of reduced speech. Poster presented at the Nijmegen Spontaneous Speech Workshop (Nijmegen, NL).
Pollack, I., & Pickett, J. M. (1963). Intelligibility of excerpts from conversational speech. Language and Speech, 6, 165–171.
Ranbom, L. J., & Connine, C. M. (2007). Lexical representation of phonological variation in spoken word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 57(2), 273–298.
Richter, E. (1930). Beobachtungen über Anglitt und Abglitt an Sprachkurven und umgekehrt laufenden Phonogrammplatten. In Paul Menzerath (ed.) Berichte über die I. Tagung der Internationalen Gesellschaft für experimentelle Phonetik, 87–90. Bonn: Scheur.
Ruiter de, L. E. (2015). Information status marking in spontaneous vs. read speech in story-telling tasks – Evidence from intonation analysis using GToBI. Journal of Phonetics 48, 29–44.
Schönle, P. W., Gräbe, K., Wenig, P., Höhne, J., Schrader, J., & Conrad, B. (1987). Electromagnetic articulography: Use of alternating magnetic fields for tracking movements of multiple points inside and outside the vocal tract. Brain and Language, 31(1), 26–35.
Schweitzer, K., Walsh, M., Calhoun, S., Schütze, H., Möbius, B., Schweitzer, A., & Dogil, G. (2015). Exploring the relationship between intonation and the lexicon: Evidence for lexicalised storage of intonation. Speech Communication, 66, 65–81.
Stampe, D. (1973). A Dissertation on Natural Phonology. PhD Diss. University of Chicago.
Stone, M. (1990). A three-dimensional model of tongue movement based on ultrasound and X-ray microbeam data. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 87(5), 2207–2217.
Taft, M., & Chen, H. C. (1992). Judging homophony in Chinese: The influence of tones. Advances in Psychology, 90, 151–172.
Tagliamonte, S. A., & Baayen, R. H. (2012). Models, forests, and trees of York English: Was/were variation as a case study for statistical practice. Language Variation and Change, 24(2), 135–178.
Torreira, F., Adda-Decker, M., and Ernestus, M. (2010). The Nijmegen Corpus of Casual French. Speech Communication, 52:201–221.
Tucker, B. V. (2007). Spoken word recognition of the reduced American English Flap. The University of Arizona. Retrieved from [URL]
Tucker, B. V. (2011). The effect of reduction on the processing of flaps and /g/ in isolated words. Journal of Phonetics, 39(3), 312–318.
Tyrone, M. E., & Mauk, C. E. (2010). Sign lowering and phonetic reduction in American Sign Language. Journal of Phonetics, 38(2), 317–328.
van Rij, J., Natalya, P., van Rijn, H., Wood, S. N., & Baayen, R. H. (submitted). Pupil dilation to study cognitive processing: challenges and solutions for time course analyses.
Ven, M. van de, Ernestus, M. & Schreuder, R. (2012). Predicting acoustically reduced words in spontaneous speech: The role of semantic/syntactic and acoustic cues in context. Laboratory Phonology 3, 455–481.
Viebahn, M., M. Ernestus, & J. McQueen. (2015). Syntactic predictability in the recognition of carefully and casually produced speech. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41 (6), 1684–1702.
Wagner, P., Trouvain, J., & Zimmerer, F. (2015). In defense of stylistic diversity in speech research. Journal of Phonetics, 48, 1–12.
Warner, N. (2011). Reduction. In M. van Oostendorp, C. Ewen, E. Hume, & K. Rice (Eds.), The Blackwell Companion to Phonology: General issues and segmental phonology (Vol. 1, 1866–1891). John Wiley & Sons.
Warner, N. (2012). Methods for studying spontaneous speech. In A. Cohn, C. Fougeron, & M. Huffman (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Laboratory Phonology. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 621–633.
Warner, N., & Tucker, B. V. (2011). Phonetic variability of stops and flaps in spontaneous and careful speech. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 130(3), 1606–1617.
Wiggers, P., & Rothkrantz, L. J. M. (2007). Exploratory analysis of word use and sentence length in the Spoken Dutch Corpus. In V. Matoušek & P. Mautner (Eds.), Text, Speech and Dialogue (366–373). Springer Berlin Heidelberg.
Willems, R. M., Frank, S. L., Nijhof, A. D., Hagoort, P., & Bosch, A. van den. (2016). Prediction during natural language comprehension. Cerebral Cortex, 26(6), 2506–2516.
Wood, S. N. (2006). Generalized Additive Models. New York: Chapman & Hall/CRC.
Wrench, A. A., & Scobbie, J. M. (2011). Very high frame rate ultrasound tongue imaging. In Proceedings of the 9th international seminar on speech production (ISSP) (pp. 155–162).
Wurm, L. H., & Fisicaro, S. A. (2014). What residualizing predictors in regression analyses does (and what it does not do). Journal of Memory and Language, 72, 37–48.
Xiong, W., Droppo, J., Huang, X., Seide, F., Seltzer, M., Stolcke, A., … Zweig, G. (2016). The Microsoft 2016 Conversational Speech Recognition System. arXiv:1609.03528 [Cs]. Retrieved from [URL]
Xu, Y. (2010). In defense of lab speech. Journal of Phonetics, 38(3), 329–336.
Zekveld, A. A., Kramer, S. E., & Festen, J. M. (2010). Pupil response as an indication of effortful listening: The influence of sentence intelligibility. Ear and Hearing, 31, 480–490.