References
Baars, B.J
(2005) Global workspace theory of consciousness: Toward a cognitive neuroscience of human experience. Progress in Brain Research, 150, 45–53. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bachmann, T
(2006) A single metatheoretical framework for a number of conscious-vision phenomena. In Q. Jing, M.R. Rosenzweig, G. d’Ydewalle, H. Zhang, H.-C. Chen, & K. Zhang (Eds.), Progress in psychological science around the world. Volume 1 Neural, cognitive and developmental issues (pp.229–242). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Bachmann, T., & Murd, C
(2010) Covert spatial attention in search for the location of a color-afterimage patch speeds up its decay from awareness: Introducing a method useful for the study of neural correlates of visual awareness. Vision Research, 50(11), 1048–1053. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bauby, J.-B
(1997) The diving bell and the butterfly (J. Leggatt, Trans.). New York, NY: Alfred A. Knopf.Google Scholar
Blake, R., & Logothetis, N.K
(2002) Visual competition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(1), 13–21. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Block, N
(2005) Two neural correlates of consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(2), 46–52. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007) Consciousness, accessibility, and the mesh between psychology and neuroscience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30(5–6), 481–499.Google Scholar
(2011) Perceptual consciousness overflows cognitive access. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(12), 567–575. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brascamp, J.W., van Boxtel, J.J.A., Knapen, T.H.J., & Blake, R
(2010) A dissociation of attention and awareness in phase-sensitive but not phase-insensitive visual channels. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(10), 2326–2344. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Braun, J
(2001) It’s great but not necessarily about attention. Psyche, 7(6). Retrieved from [URL]Google Scholar
Brown, R
(2011) The myth of phenomenological overflow. Consciousness and Cognition, 21(2), 599–604. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cartwright-Finch, U., & Lavie, N
(2007) The role of perceptual load in inattentional blindness. Cognition, 102(3), 321–340. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Castelo-Branco M., Kozak, L.R., Formisano E., Teixeira J., Xavier J., & Goebel R
(2009) Type of featural attention differentially modulates hMT + responses to illusory motion aftereffects. Journal of Neurophysiology, 102(5), 3016–3025. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cattaneo, Z., Vecchi, T., Pascual-Leone, A., & Silvanto, J
(2009) Contrasting early visual cortical activation states causally involved in visual imagery and short-term memory. European Journal of Neuroscience, 30(7), 1393–1400. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chalmers, D.J
(1996) The conscious mind: In search of a fundamental theory. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Chun, M.M., & Wolfe, J.M
(2001) Visual attention. In E.B. Goldstein (Ed.), Blackwell handbook of perception (pp.272–310). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Cohen, M.A., Alvarez, G.A., & Nakayama, K
(2011) Natural-scene perception requires attention. Psychological Science, 22(9), 1165–1172. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Coltheart, M
(1980a) Iconic memory and visible persistence. Perception & Psychophysics, 27(3), 183–228. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1980b) The persistences of vision. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 290(1038), 57–69. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
De Brigard, F., & Prinz, J
(2010) Attention and consciousness. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1(1), 51–59. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
de Gardelle, V., Sackur, J., & Kouider, S
(2009) Perceptual illusions in brief visual presentations. Consciousness and Cognition, 18(3), 569–577. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
de Graaf, T.A., de Jong, M.C., Goebel, R., van Ee, R., & Sack, A.T
(2011) On the functional relevance of frontal cortex for passive and voluntarily controlled bistable vision. Cerebral Cortex, 21(10), 2322–2331. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Deco, G., & Rolls, E.T
(2005) Attention, short-term memory, and action selection: A unifying theory. Progress in Neurobiology, 76(4), 236–256. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dehaene, S., Changeux, J.-P., Naccache, L., Sackur, J., & Sergent, C
(2006) Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: A testable taxonomy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(5), 204–211. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Enoksson, P
(1968) Studies in optokinetic binocular rivalry with a new device. Acta Ophthalmologica, 46(1), 71–74. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fei-Fei, L., VanRullen, R., Koch, C., & Perona, P
(2002) Rapid natural scene categorization in the near absence of attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 99(14), 9596–9601. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fernández-Espejo, D., & Owen, A.M
(2013) Detecting awareness after severe brain injury. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14(11), 801–809. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fox, R., Todd, S., & Bettinger, L.A
(1975) Optokinetic nystagmus as an objective indicator of binocular rivalry. Vision Research, 15(7), 849–853. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Frässle, S., Sommer, J., Jansen, A., Naber, M., & Einhäuser, W
(2014) Binocular rivalry: Frontal activity relates to introspection and action but not to perception. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(5), 1738–1747. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fries, P., Roelfsema, P.R., Engel, A.K., König, P., & Singer, W
(1997) Synchronization of oscillatory responses in visual cortex correlates with perception in interocular rivalry. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 94(23), 12699–12704. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hampshire, A., Chamberlain, S.R., Monti, M.M., Duncan, J., & Owen, A.M
(2010) The role of the right inferior frontal gyrus: Inhibition and attentional control. Neuroimage, 50(3), 1313–1319. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hardcastle, V.G
(1997) Attention versus consciousness: A distinction with a difference. Cognitive Studies, 4(3), 56–66.Google Scholar
Hayashi, R., & Tanifuji, M
(2012) Which image is in awareness during binocular rivalry? Reading perceptual status from eye movements. Journal of Vision, 12(3), 5. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Henderson, J.M., & Hollingworth, A
(1999) High-level scene perception. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 243–271. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Huk, A.C., Ress, D., & Heeger, D.J
(2001) Neuronal basis of the motion aftereffect reconsidered. Neuron, 32(1), 161–172. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hulme, O.J., Friston, K.F., & Zeki, S
(2009) Neural correlates of stimulus reportability. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(8), 1602–1610. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Imamoglu, F., Kahnt, T., Koch, C., & Haynes, J.-D
(2012) Changes in functional connectivity support conscious object recognition. Neuroimage, 63(4), 1909–1917. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Iwasaki, S
(1993) Spatial attention and two modes of visual consciousness. Cognition, 49(3), 211–233. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jack, A.I., Shulman, G.L., Snyder, A.Z., McAvoy, M., & Corbetta, M
(2006) Separate modulations of human V1 associated with spatial attention and task structure. Neuron, 51(1), 135–147. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jackendoff, R
(1996) How language helps us think. Pragmatics and Cognition, 4(1), 1–34. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jonides, J., Lewis, R.L., Nee, D.E., Lustig, C.A., Berman, M.G., & Moore, K.S
(2008) The mind and brain of short-term memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 193–224. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kanai, R., Carmel, D., Bahrami, B., & Rees, G
(2011) Structural and functional fractionation of right superior parietal cortex in bistable perception. Current Biology, 21(3), R106–R107. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kanai, R., & Rees, G
(2011) The structural basis of inter-individual differences in human behaviour and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 12(4), 231–242. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kanai, R., & Tsuchiya, N
(2012) Qualia. Current Biology, 22(10), R392–6. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kanai, R., Tsuchiya, N., & Verstraten, F.A
(2006) The scope and limits of top-down attention in unconscious visual processing. Current Biology, 16(23), 2332–2336. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kentridge, R.W., Heywood, C.A., & Weiskrantz, L
(2004) Spatial attention speeds discrimination without awareness in blindsight. Neuropsychologia, 42(6), 831–835. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Keogh, R., & Pearson, J
(2011) Mental imagery and visual working memory. PLoS ONE, 6(12), e29221. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Knapen, T., Brascamp, J., Pearson, J., van Ee, R., & Blake, R
(2011) The role of frontal and parietal brain areas in bistable perception. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(28), 10293–10301. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Koch, C
(2004) The quest for consciousness: A neurobiological approach. Englewood, CO: Roberts & Company.Google Scholar
Koch, C., & Tsuchiya, N
(2007) Attention and consciousness: Two distinct brain processes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(1), 16–22. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012) Attention and consciousness: Related yet different. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(2), 103–105. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Koivisto, M., & Silvanto, J
(2011) Relationship between visual binding, reentry and awareness. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(4), 1293–1303. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kok, P., Rahnev, D., Jehee, J.F.M., Lau, H.C., & de Lange, F.P
(2012) Attention reverses the effect of prediction in silencing sensory signals. Cerebral Cortex, 22(9), 2197–2206. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kouider, S., Stahlhut, C., Gelskov, S.V., Barbosa, L.S., Dutat, M., de Gardelle, V., Christophe, A., Dehaene, S., & Dehaene-Lambertz, G
(2013) A neural marker of perceptual consciousness in infants. Science, 340(6130), 376–380. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lamme, V.A.F
(2003) Why visual attention and awareness are different. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(1), 12–18. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2010) How neuroscience will change our view on consciousness. Cognitive Neuroscience, 1(3), 204–220. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Landman, R., Spekreijse, H., & Lamme, V.A.F
(2003) Large capacity storage of integrated objects before change blindness. Vision Research, 43(2), 149–164. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lau, H., & Rosenthal, D
(2011) Empirical support for higher-order theories of conscious awareness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(8), 365–373. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Laureys, S
(2005) The neural correlate of (un)awareness: Lessons from the vegetative state. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(12), 556–559. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lee, S.-H., Blake, R., & Heeger, D.J
(2007) Hierarchy of cortical responses underlying binocular rivalry. Nature Neuroscience, 10(8), 1048–1054. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leopold, D.A., Wilke, M., Maier, A., & Logothetis, N.K
(2002) Stable perception of visually ambiguous patterns. Nature Neuroscience, 5(6), 605–609. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Logothetis, N.K
(2002) The neural basis of the blood-oxygen-level-dependent functional magnetic resonance imaging signal. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 357(1424), 1003–1037. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lumer, E.D., Friston, K.J., & Rees, G
(1998) Neural correlates of perceptual rivalry in the human brain. Science, 280(5371), 1930–1934. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mack, A., & Rock, I
(1998) Inattentional blindness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Marchetti, G
(2012) Against the view that consciousness and attention are fully dissociable. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 36. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Merikle, P.M., & Joordens, S
(1997) Parallels between perception without attention and perception without awareness. Consciousness and Cognition, 6(2–3), 219–236. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Miller, S.M
(2007) On the correlation/constitution distinction problem (and other hard problems) in the scientific study of consciousness. Acta Neuropsychiatrica, 19(3), 159–176. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(Ed.) (2013) The constitution of visual consciousness: Lessons from binocular rivalry. Advances in Consciousness Research (Vol. 90). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins Publishing Company. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mole, C
(2008) Attention and consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 15(4), 86–104.Google Scholar
Monti, M.M., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Coleman, M.R., Boly, M., Pickard, J.D., Tshibanda, L., Owen, A.M., & Laureys, S
(2010) Willful modulation of brain activity in disorders of consciousness. New England Journal of Medicine, 362(7), 579–589. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Murd, C., & Bachmann, T
(2011) Spatially localized motion aftereffect disappears faster from awareness when selectively attended to according to its direction. Vision Research, 51(10), 1157–1162. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Naccache, L., Blandin, E., & Dehaene, S
(2002) Unconscious masked priming depends on temporal attention. Psychological Science, 13(5), 416–424. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Oberauer, K
(2002) Access to information in working memory: Exploring the focus of attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28(3), 411–421. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Oizumi, M., Albantakis, L., & Tononi, G
(2014) From the phenomenology to the mechanisms of consciousness: Integrated Information Theory 3.0. PLoS Computational Biology, 10(5), e1003588. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
O’Regan, J.K
(1992) Solving the “real” mysteries of visual perception: The world as an outside memory. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 46(3), 461–488. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
O’Regan, J.K., & Noë, A
(2001) A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(5), 939–973. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Owen, A.M., Coleman, M.R., Boly, M., Davis, M.H., Laureys, S., & Pickard, J.D
(2006) Detecting awareness in the vegetative state. Science, 313(5792), 1402. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pessoa, L., & Padmala, S
(2005) Quantitative prediction of perceptual decisions during near-threshold fear detection. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 102(15), 5612–5617. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Phillips, I.B
(2011) Perception and iconic memory: What Sperling doesn’t show. Mind & Language, 26(4), 381–411. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pinto, Y., Sligte, I.G., Shapiro, K.L., & Lamme, V.A.F
(2013) Fragile visual short-term memory is an object-based and location-specific store. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20(4), 732–739. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Polonsky, A., Blake, R., Braun, J., & Heeger, D.J
(2000) Neuronal activity in human primary visual cortex correlates with perception during binocular rivalry. Nature Neuroscience, 3(11), 1153–1159. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Posner, M.I
(1994) Attention: The mechanisms of consciousness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 91(16), 7398–7403.
Prinz, J
(2010) When is perception conscious? In B. Nanay (Ed.), Perceiving the world: New essays on perception (pp.310–332). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ramsøy, T.Z., & Overgaard, M
(2004) Introspection and subliminal perception. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 3(1), 1–23. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rensink, R.A
(2002) Change detection. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 245–277. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rensink, R.A., O’Regan, J.K., & Clark, J.J
(1997) To see or not to see: The need for attention to perceive changes in scenes. Psychological Science, 8(5), 368–373. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ress, D., & Heeger, D.J
(2003) Neuronal correlates of perception in early visual cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 6(4), 414–420. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ricci, C., & Blundo, C
(1990) Perception of ambiguous figures after focal brain lesions. Neuropsychologia, 28(11), 1163–1173. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sampanes, A.C., Tseng, P., & Bridgeman, B
(2008) The role of gist in scene recognition. Vision Research, 48(21), 2275–2283. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Siclari, F., Larocque, J.J., Postle, B.R., & Tononi, G
(2013) Assessing sleep consciousness within subjects using a serial awakening paradigm. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 542. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sligte, I.G., Scholte, H.S., & Lamme, V.A.F
(2008) Are there multiple visual short-term memory stores? PLoS ONE, 3(2), e1699. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sperling, G
(1960) The information available in brief visual presentations. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 74(11), 1–29. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Srinivasan, N., Srivastava, P., Lohani, M., & Baijal, S
(2009) Focused and distributed attention. Progress in Brain Research, 176, 87–100. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sterzer, P., & Kleinschmidt, A
(2007) A neural basis for inference in perceptual ambiguity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 104(1), 323–328. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sterzer, P., Kleinschmidt, A., & Rees, G
(2009) The neural bases of multistable perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(7), 310–318. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Summerfield, C., & Egner, T
(2009) Expectation (and attention) in visual cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(9), 403–409. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Swick, D., Ashley, V., & Turken, A.U
(2008) Left inferior frontal gyrus is critical for response inhibition. BMC Neuroscience, 9, 102. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tong, F., & Engel, S.A
(2001) Interocular rivalry revealed in the human cortical blind-spot representation. Nature, 411(6834), 195–199. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tong, F., Meng, M., & Blake, R
(2006) Neural bases of binocular rivalry. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(11), 502–511. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tononi, G
(2010) Information integration: Its relevance to brain function and consciousness. Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 148(3), 299–322.Google Scholar
Tootell, R.B.H., Reppas, J.B., Dale, A.M., Look, R.B., Sereno, M.I., Malach, R., Brady, T.J., & Rosen, B.R
(1995) Visual motion aftereffects in human cortical area MT revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Nature, 375(6527), 139–141. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Treisman, A., & Schmidt, H
(1982) Illusory conjunctions in the perception of objects. Cognitive Psychology, 14(1), 107–141. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tse, P.U
(2004) Mapping visual attention with change blindness: New directions for a new method. Cognitive Science, 28(2), 241–258. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tse, P.U., Martinez-Conde, S., Schlegel, A.A., & Macknik, S.L
(2005) Visibility, visual awareness, and visual masking of simple unattended targets are confined to areas in the occipital cortex beyond human V1/V2. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 102(47), 17178–17183. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tsuchiya, N., & Koch, C
(2008a) Attention and consciousness. Scholarpedia, 3(5), 4173. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2008b) The relationship between consciousness and attention. In S. Laureys & G. Tononi (Eds.), The neurology of consciousness: Cognitive neuroscience and neuropathology (pp.63–77). London, UK: Academic Press.Google Scholar
van Boxtel, J.J.A., Tsuchiya, N., & Koch, C
(2010a) Consciousness and attention: On sufficiency and necessity. Frontiers in Consciousness Research, 1, 217. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2010b) Opposing effects of attention and consciousness on afterimages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 107(19), 8883–8888. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vandenbroucke, A.R.E., Sligte, I.G., Barrett, A.B., Seth, A.K., Fahrenfort, J.J., & Lamme, V.A.F
(2014) Accurate metacognition for visual sensory memory representations. Psychological Science, 25(4), 861–873. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vandenbroucke, A.R.E., Sligte, I.G., & Lamme, V.A.F
(2011) Manipulations of attention dissociate fragile visual short-term memory from visual working memory. Neuropsychologia, 49(6), 1559–1568. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Velmans, M
(Ed.) (1996) The science of consciousness: Psychological, neuropsychological and clinical reviews. London, UK: Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Watanabe, M., Cheng, K., Murayama, Y., Ueno, K., Asamizuya, T., Tanaka, K., & Logothetis, N
(2011) Attention but not awareness modulates the BOLD signal in the human V1 during binocular suppression. Science, 334(6057), 829–831. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wilke, M., Logothetis, N.K., & Leopold, D.A
(2003) Generalized flash suppression of salient visual targets. Neuron, 39(6), 1043–1052. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wilke, M., Mueller, K.M., & Leopold, D.A
(2009) Neural activity in the visual thalamus reflects perceptual suppression. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 106(23), 9465–9470. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wolfe, J.M
(1999) Inattentional amnesia. In V. Coltheart (Ed.), Fleeting memories: Cognition of brief visual stimuli (pp.71–94). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Wolfe, J.M., Horowitz, T.S., & Kenner, N.M
(2005) Cognitive psychology: Rare items often missed in visual searches. Nature, 435(7041), 439–440. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wolfe, J.M., Reinecke, A., & Brawn, P
(2006) Why don’t we see changes?: The role of attentional bottlenecks and limited visual memory. Visual Cognition, 14(4–8), 749–780. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Woodman, G.F., & Luck, S.J
(2003) Dissociations among attention, perception, and awareness during object-substitution masking. Psychological Science, 14(6), 605–611. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wundt, W
(1874) Grundzüge der physiologischen Psychologie. Leipzig: Engelmann.Google Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 2 other publications

Davidson, Matthew J, Will Mithen, Hinze Hogendoorn, Jeroen JA van Boxtel & Naotsugu Tsuchiya
2020. The SSVEP tracks attention, not consciousness, during perceptual filling-in. eLife 9 DOI logo
Thomas, Victoria, Matthew Davidson, Parisa Zakavi, Naotsugu Tsuchiya & Jeroen van Boxtel
2017. Simulated forward and backward self motion, based on realistic parameters, causes motion induced blindness. Scientific Reports 7:1 DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 19 march 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.