Part of
Visually Situated Language Comprehension
Edited by Pia Knoeferle, Pirita Pyykkönen-Klauck and Matthew W. Crocker
[Advances in Consciousness Research 93] 2016
► pp. 227260
References
Abashidze, D., Carminati, M.N., & Knoeferle, P
(2014) How robust is the recent-event preference? In P. Bello, M. Guarini, M. McShane, & B. Scassellati (Eds.), Proceedings of the 36th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 92-97). Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Abashidze, D., Knoeferle, P., Carminati, M.N., & Essig, K
(2011) The role of recent real-world versus future events in the comprehension of referentially ambiguous sentences: Evidence from eye tracking. In B. Kokinov, A. Karmiloff-Smith, & N.J. Nersessian (Eds.), Proceedings of the European Conference on Cognitive Science. New Bulgarian University Press.Google Scholar
Allopenna, P., Magnuson, J., & Tanenhaus, M
(1998) Tracking the time course of spoken word recognition using eye movements: Evidence for continuous mapping models. Journal of Memory & Language, 38, 419-439. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Altmann, G.T.M
(2004) Language-mediated eye-movements in the absence of a visual world: The ‘blank screen paradigm’. Cognition, 93, B79-B87. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Altmann, G.T.M., & Kamide, Y
(1999) Incremental interpretation at verbs: Restricting the domain of subsequent reference. Cognition, 73, 247-264. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2004) Now you see it, now you don’t: Mediating the mapping between language and the visual world. In J. Henderson & F. Ferreira (Eds.), The integration of language, vision and action (pp. 347-386). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
(2007) The real-time mediation of visual attention by language and world knowledge: Linking anticipatory (and other) eye movements to linguistic processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 57, 502-518. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009) Discourse-mediation of the mapping between language and the visual world: Eye movements and mental representation. Cognition, 111, 55-71. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Andersson, R., Ferreira, F., & Henderson, J.M
(2011) I see what you’re saying: The integration of complex speech and scenes during language comprehension. Acta Psychologica, 137, 208-216. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Arai, M., van Gompel, R., & Scheepers, C
(2007) Priming ditransitive structures in comprehension. Cognitive Psychology, 54, 218-250. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Arbib, M
(2005) From monkey-like action recognition to human language: An evolutionary framework for neurolinguistics. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 105-167. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barsalou, L.W
(1999) Perceptual and symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 577-609.Google Scholar
Brennan, S., Chen, X., Dickinson, C., Neider, M., & Zelinsky, G
(2007) Coordinating cognition: The costs and benefits of shared gaze during collaborative search. Cognition, 106, 1465-1477. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Burigo, M., & Knoeferle, P
(2011) Visual attention during spatial language comprehension: Reference alone isn’t enough. In L. Carlson, C. Holscher, & T. Shipley (Eds.), Proceedings of the 33rd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Carminati, M.N., & Knoeferle, P
(2013) Effects of speaker emotional facial expression and listener age on incremental sentence processing. PLoS ONE, 8, e72559. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carstensen, L.L., Fung, H.H., & Charles, S.T
(2003) Socioemotional selectivity theory and the regulation of emotion in the second half of life. Motivation and Emotion, 27, 103-123. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Casasanto, C
(2008) Similarity and proximity: When does close in space mean close in mind? Memory & Cognition, 36, 1047-1056. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Castelhano, M., Mack, M., & Henderson, J
(2009) Viewing task influences eye movement control during active scene perception. Journal of Vision, 9(3), 1-15. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chambers, C.G., & San Juan, V
(2008) Perception and presupposition in real-time language comprehension: Insights from anticipatory processing. Cognition, 108, 26-50. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chambers, C.G., Tanenhaus, M.K., Filip, H., & Carlson, G.N
(2002) Circumscribing referential domains during real time language comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 47, 30-49. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chambers, C.G., Tanenhaus, M.K., & Magnuson, J.S
(2004) Actions and affordances in syntactic ambiguity resolution. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 687-696. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cook, S.W., & Tanenhaus, M.K
(2009) Embodied communication: Speakers’ gestures affect listeners’ actions. Cognition, 113, 98-104. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cooper, R
(1974) The control of eye fixation by the meaning of spoken language: A new methodology for the real-time investigation of speech perception, memory, and language processing. Cognitive Psychology, 6, 84-107. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crocker, M.W., Knoeferle, P., & Mayberry, M
(2010) Situated sentence comprehension: The coordinated interplay account and a neurobehavioral model. Brain and Language, 112, 189-201. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dahan, D., & Tanenhaus, M
(2005) Looking at the rope when looking for the snake: Conceptually mediated eye movements during spoken-word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 453-459. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Duñabeitia, J.A., Aviles, A., Afonso, O., Scheepers, C., & Carreiras, M
(2008) Qualitative differences in the representation of abstract versus concrete words: Evidence from the visual-world paradigm. Cognition, 110, 284-292. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Engelhardt, P.E., Ferreira, F., & Patsenko, E.G
(2010) Pupillometry reveals processing load during spoken language comprehension. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 639-664. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ferreira, F., Foucart, A., & Engelhardt, P.E
(2013) Language processing in the visual world: Effects of preview, visual complexity, and prediction. Journal of Memory and Language, 69, 165-182. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J
(1983) Modularity of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Frazier, L., & Fodor, J.D
(1978) The sausage machine: A new two-stage parsing model. Cognition, 6, 291-325. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Friederici, A.D
(2002) Towards a neural basis of auditory sentence processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 78-84. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ganis, G., Kutas, M., & Sereno, M.I
(1996) The search for ”common sense”: An electrophysiological study of the comprehension of words and pictures in reading. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8, 89-106. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goolkasian, P
(1996) Picture-word differences in a sentence verification task. Memory & Cognition, 24, 584-594. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Griffin, Z., & Bock, K
(2000) What the eyes say about speaking. Psychological Science, 11, 274-279. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Guerra, E., & Knoeferle, P
(2014) Effects of object distance on incremental semantic interpretation: Similarity is closeness. Cognition, 133, 535-552. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Habets, B., Kita, S., Shao, Z., Ozyurek, A., & Hagoort, P
(2011) The role of synchrony and ambiguity in speech-gesture integration during comprehension. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 1845-1854. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hagoort, P., Brown, C., & Groothusen, J
(1993) The syntactic positive shift (sps) as an ERP measure of syntactic processing. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8, 439-483. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hanna, J., & Brennan, S
(2007) Speakers’ eye gaze disambiguates referring expressions early during face-to-face conversation. Journal of Memory and Language, 57, 596-615. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Henderson, J.M., & Ferreira, F
(2004) Scene perception for psycholinguists. In J.M. Henderson & F. Ferreira (Eds.), The interface of language, vision, and action: Eye movements and the visual world (pp. 1-58). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Huang, Y.T., & Snedeker, J
(2009) Online interpretation of scalar quantifiers: Insight into the semantics –pragmatics interface. Cognitive Psychology, 58, 376-415. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Huettig, F., & Altmann, G.T.M
(2005) Word meaning and the control of eye fixation: Semantic competitor effects and the visual world paradigm. Cognition, 96, 23-32. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Huettig, F., Rommers, J., & Meyer, A.S
(2011) Using the visual world paradigm to study language processing: A review and critical evaluation. Acta Psychologica, 137, 151-171. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Isaacowitz, D.M., Allard, E.S., Murphy, N.A., & Schlangel, M
(2009) The time course of age-related preferences toward positive and negative stimuli. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 64B, 188-192. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kelly, S.D., Barr, D.J., Church, R.B., & Lynch, K
(1999) Offering a hand to pragmatic understanding: The role of speech and gesture in comprehension and memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 40, 577-592. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kelly, S.D., Creigh, P., & Bartolotti, J
(2010) Integrating speech and iconic gestures in a stroop-like task: Evidence for automatic processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 683-694. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kelly, S.D., Ozyurek, A., & Maris, E
(2010) Two sides of the same coin: Speech and gesture mutually interact to enhance comprehension. Psychological Science, 21, 260-267. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Knoeferle, P
(2007) Comparing the time-course of processing initially ambiguous and unambiguous German SVO/OVS sentences in depicted events. In R. van Gompel, M. Fischer, W. Murray, & R. Hill (Eds.), Eye movement research: Insights into mind and brain (pp. 517-533). Oxford: Elsevier. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
in pressa). Language comprehension in rich non-linguistic contexts: Combining eye tracking and event-related brain potentials. In Towards a cognitive neuroscience of natural language use. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logo
in pressb). Visually situated language comprehension in children and in adults. In R.K. Mishra, N. Srinivasan, & F. Huettig (Eds.) Attention and vision in language processing Springer Language and Cognition series DOI logo
Knoeferle, P., Carminati, M.N., Abashidze, D., & Essig, K
(2011) Preferential inspection of recent real-world events over future events: Evidence from eye tracking during spoken sentence comprehension. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 376. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Knoeferle, P., & Crocker, M.W
(2005) Incremental effects of mismatch during picture-sentence integration: Evidence from eye-tracking. In Proceedings of the 27th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Conference (pp. 1166-1171). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
(2006) The coordinated interplay of scene, utterance, and world knowledge: Evidence from eye tracking. Cognitive Science, 30, 481-529. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007) The influence of recent scene events on spoken comprehension: Evidence from eye-movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 75, 519-543. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Knoeferle, P., Crocker, M.W., Scheepers, C., & Pickering, M.J
(2005) The influence of the immediate visual context on incremental thematic role-assignment: Evidence from eye-movements in depicted events. Cognition, 95, 95-127. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Knoeferle, P., Habets, B., Crocker, M.W., & Munte, T.F
(2008) Visual scenes trigger immediate syntactic reanalysis: Evidence from ERPs during situated spoken comprehension. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 789-795. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Knoeferle, P., & Kreysa, H
(2012) Effects of speaker gaze on syntactic structuring. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 376. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Knoeferle, P., Urbach, T.P., & Kutas, M
(2011) Comprehending how visual context influences incremental sentence comprehension: Insights from ERPs and picture-sentence verification. Psychophysiology, 48, 495-506. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Knoeferle, P., Urbach, T., & Kutas, M
(2014) Different mechanisms for role relations versus verb-action congruence effects: Evidence from ERPs in picture-sentence verification. Acta Psychologica, 152, 133-148. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kolk, H., Chwilla, D., Van Herten, M., & Oor, P
(2003) Structure and limited capacity in verbal working memory: A study with event-related potentials. Brain and Language, 85, 1-36. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kreysa, H
(2009) Coordinating speech-related eye movements between comprehension and production. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Edinburgh, UK.Google Scholar
Kreysa, H., & Knoeferle, P
(2011a) Effects of speaker gaze on spoken language comprehension: Task matters. In L. Carlson, C. Holscher, & T. Shipley (Eds.), Proceedings of the 33rd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
(2011b) Peripheral speaker gaze facilitates spoken language comprehension: Syntactic structuring and thematic role assignment in German. In B. Kokinov, A. Karmiloff-Smith, & N.J. Nersessian (Eds.), Proceedings of the European Conference on Cognitive Science. New Bulgarian University Press.Google Scholar
Kreysa, H., Knoeferle, P., & Nunnemann, E
(2014) Effects of speaker gaze versus depicted actions on visual attention during sentence comprehension. In M. McShane, B. Scassellati, P. Bello, & M. Guarini (Eds.), Proceedings of the 36th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 2513-2518). Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Kuperberg, G., Sitnikova, T., Caplan, D., & Holcomb, P
(2003) Electrophysiological distinctions in processing conceptual relationships within simple sentences. Cognitive Brain Research, 17, 117-129. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K.D
(2011) Thirty years and counting: Finding meaning in the N400 component of the event related brain potential (ERP). Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 621-647. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S.A
(1980) Reading senseless sentences: Brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science, 207, 203-205. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1984) Brain potentials during reading reflect word expectancy and semantic association. Nature, 307, 161-163. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kutas, M., Van Petten, C., & Kluender, R
(2006) In M. Traxler & M. Gerns-bacher (Eds.), Handbook of psycholinguistics (2nd ed., pp. 659-724). New York: Elsevier. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mayberry, M., Crocker, M.W., & Knoeferle, P
(2009) Learning to attend: A connectionist model of situated language comprehension. Cognitive Science, 33, 449-496. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nappa, R., & Arnold, J
(2009) Paying attention to intention: Effects of intention (but not egocentric attention) on pronoun resolution. In Proceedings of the CUNY Conference (p. 262).
Nation, K., & Altmann, C.M.M.G.T.M
(2003) Investigating individual differences in childrens real-time sentence comprehension using language-mediated eye movements. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 86, 314-329. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Osterhout, L., & Holcomb, P.J
(1992) Event-related brain potentials elicited by syntactic anomaly. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 785-806. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pulvermuller, F., Harle, M., & Hummel, F
(2001) Walking or talking?: Behavioral and neurophysiological correlates of action verb processing. Brain and Language, 78, 143-168. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Richardson, D., & Matlock, T
(2007) The integration of figurative language and static depictions: An eye movement study of fictive motion. Cognition, 102, 129-138. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Richardson, D.C., & Dale, R
(2005) Looking to understand: The coupling between speakers’ and listeners’ eye movements and its relationship to discourse comprehension. Cognitive Science, 29, 1045-1060. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Richardson, D.C., & Kirkham, N.Z
(2004) Multi-modal events and moving locations: Eye movements of adults and 6-month-olds reveal dynamic spatial indexing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 46-62. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Salverda, A.P., Brown, M., & Tanenhaus, M.K
(2011) A goal-based perspective on eye movements in visual-world studies. Acta Psychologica, 137, 172-180. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Scheepers, C., & Crocker, M.W
(2004) Constituent order priming from reading to listening: A visual-world study. In M. Carreiras & J.C. Clifton (Eds.), The on-line study of sentence comprehension: Eyetracking, ERP, and beyond. United Kingdom: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Sedivy, J.C., Tanenhaus, M.K., Chambers, C.G., & Carlson, G.N
(1999) Achieving incremental semantic interpretation through contextual representation. Cognition, 71, 109-148. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Spivey, M.J., & Geng, J.J
(2001) Oculomotor mechanisms activated by imagery and memory: Eye movements to absent objects. Psychological Research, 65, 235-241. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Spivey, M.J., Tanenhaus, M.K., Eberhard, K.M., & Sedivy, J.C
(2002) Eye-movements and spoken language comprehension: Effects of visual context on syntactic ambiguity resolution. Cognitive Psychology, 45, 447-481. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Staudte, M., & Crocker, M
(2009) The effect of robot gaze on processing robot utterances. In N. Taatgen & H. van Rijn (Eds.), Proceedings of the Cognitive Science Conference (pp. 431–436). Cognitive Science Society, Inc.Google Scholar
Staudte, M., Crocker, M.W., Heloir, A., & Kipp, M
(2014) The influence of speaker gaze on listener comprehension: Contrasting visual versus intentional accounts. Cognition, 133, 317-328. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tanenhaus, M.K
(2004) That’s not it and "its" not "that": The role of conceptual composites in in-line reference resolution. In M. Carreiras & J. Charles Clifton (Eds.), On-line sentence processing: ERPs, eye movements, and beyond (pp. 209-228). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Tanenhaus, M.K., Magnuson, J.S., Dahan, D., & Chambers, C
(2000) Eye movements and lexical access in spoken- language comprehension: Evaluating a linking hypothesis between fixations and linguistic processing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 29, 557-580. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tanenhaus, M.K., Spivey-Knowlton, M.J., Eberhard, K., & Sedivy, J.C
(1995) Integration of visual and linguistic information in spoken language comprehension. Science, 268, 632-634. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tatler, B., Wade, N., Kwan, H., Findlay, J., & Velichkovsky, B
(2010) Yarbus, eye movements, and vision. I-Perception, 1, 7-27. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Underwood, G., Jebbett, L., & Roberts, K
(2004) Inspecting pictures for information to verify a sentence: Eye movements in general encoding and in focused search. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56, 165-182. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vissers, C., Kolk, H., van de Meerendonk, N., & Chwilla, D
(2008) Monitoring in language perception: Evidence from erps in a picture-sentence matching task. Neuropsychologia, 967-982. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wassenaar, M., & Hagoort, P
(2007) Thematic role assignment in patients with broca’s aphasia: Sentence-picture matching electrified. Neuropsychologia, 45, 716-740. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wu, Y.C., & Coulson, S
(2005) Meaningful gestures: Electrophysiological indices of iconic gesture comprehension. Psychophysiology, 42, 654-667. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yarbus, A.L
(1967) Eye movements and vision. New York: Plenum Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zhang, L., & Knoeferle, P
(2012) Visual context effects on thematic role assignment in children versus adults: Evidence from eye tracking in German. In R.P. Cooper, N. Miyake, & D. Peebles (Eds.), Proceedings of the annual meeting of the cognitive science society (pp. 2593-2598). Boston, USA: The Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 1 other publications

Hernández-Gutiérrez, David, Francisco Muñoz, Jose Sánchez-García, Werner Sommer, Rasha Abdel Rahman, Pilar Casado, Laura Jiménez-Ortega, Javier Espuny, Sabela Fondevila & Manuel Martín-Loeches
2021. Situating language in a minimal social context: how seeing a picture of the speaker’s face affects language comprehension. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 16:5  pp. 502 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 7 september 2023. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.